2021
DOI: 10.1186/s41235-021-00275-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When more is more: redundant modifiers can facilitate visual search

Abstract: According to the Gricean Maxim of Quantity, speakers provide the amount of information listeners require to correctly interpret an utterance, and no more (Grice in Logic and conversation, 1975). However, speakers do tend to violate the Maxim of Quantity often, especially when the redundant information improves reference precision (Degen et al. in Psychol Rev 127(4):591–621, 2020). Redundant (non-contrastive) information may facilitate real-world search if it narrows the spatial scope under consideration, or im… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Speakers are more likely to redundantly encode color than other kinds of adjectives (see Rubio‐Fernández, 2016, 2019; Tarenskeen et al., 2015, i.a. ), and listeners are more likely to prefer overspecifications for color than for other features (see Fukumura & Carminati, 2021; Rehring et al., 2021; Sedivy et al., 1999). In Experiment 2, we also observed a preference for color versus pattern redundant adjectives.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Speakers are more likely to redundantly encode color than other kinds of adjectives (see Rubio‐Fernández, 2016, 2019; Tarenskeen et al., 2015, i.a. ), and listeners are more likely to prefer overspecifications for color than for other features (see Fukumura & Carminati, 2021; Rehring et al., 2021; Sedivy et al., 1999). In Experiment 2, we also observed a preference for color versus pattern redundant adjectives.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The question is, therefore, raised: How does the use of redundant adjectives influence listeners’ referential processing? Empirical studies have to date provided mixed evidence (Arts et al., 2011a; Brodbeck et al., 2015; Davies & Katsos 2013; Engelhardt et al., 2006, 2011; Fukumura & Carminati, 2021; Rehring et al., 2021; Rubio‐Fernández, 2020; Sedivy et al., 1999; Tourtouri et al., 2019, i.a.). These studies, however, vary in important aspects, such as the referential set size (mostly using limited contexts of up to four referents), or the kind of adjectives used redundantly (usually color and size adjectives).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…target), thus favouring a top-down interpretation of the results. In addition to this point, instructions can encourage participants to adopt an attentional set for specific stimulus features, resulting in more efficient goaldriven selection (Rehrig et al, 2021;Most, Scholl et al, 2005;Aimola Davies et al, 2008;Vuilleumier & Rafal, 2000). Therefore, it is expected that experiments which instructed stimulus identification, versus simple stimulus location, would result in a greater motivational detection advantage, due to the greater top-down awareness/expectation of the specific stimulus features.…”
Section: Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…What has been found is that in interactive, spontaneous speech, speakers frequently modify nouns with adjectives that are not strictly necessary for referent identification (e.g., referring to a cup as the red cup, in a context where there are no other cups of any color) (Engelhardt et al, 2006;Nadig & Sedivy, 2002). Studies also showed that overinformative descriptions are often easily tolerated by comprehenders and can in fact be helpful for comprehension, when they describe non-canonical properties, or properties which may speed up object identification (Engelhardt et al, 2006;Rubio-Fernández (2016); Pogue et al, 2016;Arts, Maes, Noordman, & Jansen, 2011;Rehrig, Cullimore, Henderson, & Ferreira, 2021;Long, Rohde, & Rubio-Fernandez, 2020;Mangold & Pobel, 1988;Paraboni, Van Deemter, & Masthoff, 2007;Paraboni & van Deemter, 2014;Sonnenschein & Whitehurst, 1982;Tourtouri, Delogu, Sikos, & Crocker, 2019). There is, however, also evidence that informationally redundant utterances which have no apparent (e.g., perceptual) utility are unlikely to be produced, are generally judged to be relatively infelicitous, and tend to generate inferences (Davies & Katsos, 2010;Sedivy, 2003).…”
Section: Informational Redundancymentioning
confidence: 99%