2018
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-018-1588-6
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why are you looking at me? It’s because I’m talking, but mostly because I’m staring or not doing much

Abstract: Our attention is particularly driven toward faces, especially the eyes, and there is much debate over the factors that modulate this social attentional orienting. Most of the previous research has presented faces in isolation, and we tried to address this shortcoming by measuring people’s eye movements whilst they observe more naturalistic and varied social interactions. Participants’ eye movements were monitored whilst they watched three different types of social interactions (monologue, manual activity, acti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, participants looked relatively more at the arms of the walkers handing out a flyer or standing still in the hallway (see Appendix C) than they did for walkers who did not reach out an arm. We thus extend previous research that has shown that participants look at areas of the body of a person depending on what that person does (Scott et al, 2019). Critically, we show that this is also the case when participants share the same physical space with their passers-by and thus can interact with them in a direct way, as opposed to when they observe others in a video.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, participants looked relatively more at the arms of the walkers handing out a flyer or standing still in the hallway (see Appendix C) than they did for walkers who did not reach out an arm. We thus extend previous research that has shown that participants look at areas of the body of a person depending on what that person does (Scott et al, 2019). Critically, we show that this is also the case when participants share the same physical space with their passers-by and thus can interact with them in a direct way, as opposed to when they observe others in a video.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…(2012) proposed that looking behavior to faces can be considered an information-seeking process where observers seek out parts of the scene that are relevant. In a similar vein, Scott, Batten, and Kuhn (2019) presented participants with videos of an actor carrying out various behaviors (holding a monologue, manual actions, and misdirection). They showed that the actor's face was looked at most during monologues, while the actor's hands were looked at most during manual actions and misdirection.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, Foulsham and Sanderson ( 2013 ) showed that this also occurred for videos from which the sound was removed. In another study, Scott et al, ( 2019 ) showed observers three videos of an actor carrying out a monologue, manual actions (how to make a cup of tea) and misdirection (a magic trick ‘cups and balls’). They reported that faces were looked at most during monologues, but hands were looked at much more often during manual actions and misdirections in videos portrayed by an actor.…”
Section: Face Scanningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As noted before, however, information for speech recognition need not be confined to the mouth (Lansing and McConkie, 1999 ; Yehia et al, 1998 ). Finally, Scott et al, ( 2019 ) showed that eye contact by the actor (during manual activity and misdirection in particular) increased observers’ fixation time to the face.…”
Section: Face Scanningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experimentally, it has been reported that social cues, beyond gaze location, can manipulate the audience’s attention effectively ( Kuhn & Tatler, 2005 ; Kuhn et al, 2008a ; Kuhn, Amlani & Rensink, 2008b ). In general, social cues strengthen the effectiveness of the magic effects, can be imposed on explicit instructions and allow manipulating the audience’s expectations ( Cui et al, 2011 ; Hergovich & Oberfichtner, 2016 ; Kuhn & Land, 2006 ; Kuhn, Tatler & Cole, 2009 ; Kuhn & Teszka, 2015 ; Kuhn et al, 2016 ; Kuhn & Rensink, 2016 ; Rieiro, Martinez-Conde & Macknik, 2013 ; Scott, Batten & Kuhn, 2019 ; Tachibana & Kawabata, 2014 ; Thomas & Didierjean, 2016a , 2016b ; Tompkins, Woods & Aimola Davies, 2016 ).…”
Section: Survey Methodologymentioning
confidence: 99%