Four experiments investigated the role of verbal coding on retention of nonsense shapes as a function of memory load and retention interval. In training, 5s in the Named and Unnamed conditions had equal practice discriminating shapes in a delayed matching-to-sample task, while 5s in the Named condition also learned names for the shapes. A recognition test of the shapes followed pretraining. The Named condition, in general, was superior in terms of recognition performance. The advantage increased as the number of shapes to be remembered increased, but was constant across retention intervals from .2 to 50 sec. The superiority of the Named condition can be reduced or eliminated by interfering with rehearsal (counting backwards).Furthermore, estimates of shape recency in a continuous recognition task were superior for the Named condition only on items within the range of short-term memory. These data suggest that verbal coding of visual stimuli has its primary effect on recognition memory via increased rehearsal efficiency in short-term memory.
Subjects instructed to think of novel shapes in terms of relevant names made fewer errors in recalling a serial ordering of the shapes, but more errors in solving a mental jigsaw puzzle and in drawing the shapes from memory, than subjects instructed to visualize the shapes without using words.
Redintegrative memory for novel shapes (remembering the whole shape when only a part is presented) was studied as a function of whether or not S had learned names for the shapes. Naming markedly facilitated short-term redintegrative recognition, but had no effect on simple recognition. The results support the hypothesis that naming facilitates the formation of an integrated representation in which information about the different contours of the shape is more closely linked than in a nonverbal representation. Spiker (1956), Ranken (1963), and others have obtained facilitative effects of stimulus naming in a variety of memory tasks. Ss' comments during post-experimental questioning suggest that with unfamiliar shapes one effect of names is to provide stable, integrated representations of the shapes: Ss often report that the names give them "something concrete" to remember (Ranken, 1963). An integrated representation of a complex shape may be thought of as one in which the representations of the various parts or contours of the shape are closely linked, so that the occurrence of one tends to evoke the others. On this interpretation, the degree of integration of S's representation would be reflected in a redintegrative memory task, in which he is shown part of a shape and asked to remember the rest. In the present experiment, the hypothesis that naming facilitates the formation of integrated representations of unfamiliar shapes was tested by determining the effect of naming on performance in a redintegrative recognition task. METHOD StimuliThe stimulus shapes ( Fig. I) had irregular top and bottom contours and straight sides. Thus each shape could be divided by a horizontal line into a top and bottom half, each half containing one of the two contours which defined the shape. The shapes, I in. wide, were shown on a rear-projection screen at a viewing distance of about 20 in. DesignTwo training conditions, Named and Unnamed, and two test tasks, Redintegration and Recognition, were used in a 2 by 2 design. Two replications were run, approximately a month apart, with four Ss (introductory psychology students) in each cell in each replication.During training, Ss in the Named condition learned names for the shapes, while Ss in the Unnamed condition had equal practice in discriminating both top and bottom contours of the shapes in a delayed matching-ta-sample task, but were not given names. In the Redintegration test task, in essence, S was shown a top contour, followed by a bottom contour, and was asked to indicate whether the second contour came from the same shape as the first. The task may be thought of as a recognition test of redintegrative memory, in which S's ability to remember the correct bottom contour of a shape, given the top contour, is tested by his ability to recognize the presented bottom contour as correct or reject it as incorrect.The Recognition test task served as a control for the effect of naming on memory for specific contours. In essence, S was shown a top contour, followed by another top contour, ...
The effects of learning relevant names for random shapes on subsequent serial, position, and recognition learning were investigated in a series of four experiments. Name learning facilitated all three kinds of subsequent learning. The effect of naming was greater when an ordering of the shapes was learned as a temporal sequence (serial learning) than when it was learned as a spatial arrangement (position learning). Position learning was more rapid than serial learning, and the difference was greater for unnamed than for named shapes. Serial learning was as rapid with named shapes as with the names alone. Naming facilitated position learning even after Named and Unnamed groups had met the same criterion in recognition learning. Implications for various hypotheses concerning the mechanisms underlying the effects of naming are discussed. The results are interpreted as suggesting separate effects of naming on discriminability and on ease of association. The bearing of the findings on the question of the effective stimulus in serial learning is also considered.
NOTICES When Government drawings, specifications, or other data are used for any purpose other than in connection with a definitely related Government procurement operation, the United States Government thereby incurs no responsibility nor any obligation whatsoever; and the fact that the Government may have formulated, furnished, or in any way supplied the said drawings, specifications, or other data, is not to be regardedby implication or otherwise as in any manner licensing the holder or any other person or corporation, or conveying any rights or permission to manufacture, use, or sell any patented invention that may in any way be related thereto. The information furnished herewith is made available for study upon the understanding that the Government's proprietary interests in and relating thereto shall not be impaired. It is desired that the Office of the Judge Advocate (WCJ), Wright Air Development Center, Wright-PattersonAFB, Dayton, Ohio, be promptly notified of any apparent conflict between the Government's proprietary interests and those of others. The U.S. Government is absolved from any litigation which may ensue from the contractor's infringing on the foreign patent rights which may be involved.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.