JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.. Linguistic Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Language. This article presents the results of two studies which show that gender bias and stereotyping are widespread in the example sentences of syntax textbooks. Results from both studies indicate that little has changed over the past twenty-five years: virtually all of the authors favor malegendered NPs as subjects and agents and regularly stereotype both genders. Throughout the paper we make reference to the LSA guidelines for nonsexist usage, pointing out the need for such guidelines, and highlighting the gaps in the current version.* INTRODUCTION. It is a familiar and long-standing tradition to use the names John and Mary in syntactic examples. Over the last two decades, however, some syntacticians have switched to gender-neutral names such as Pat, Kim, Chris, and Sandy to avoid both the stereotypical gender roles which John and Mary virtually always play, as well as the common imbalance between use of female and male names. One might therefore assume that inequality in example sentences is no longer an issue-that linguists have largely eliminated gender bias and stereotyping from their examples. In this article we demonstrate that, unfortunately, this is not the case.We present here the results of two studies that support this claim. First, we review the results of our case study of the example sentences in one recent syntax textbook (Macaulay & Brice 1994). In that study we considered such factors as the frequency of female-vs. male-gendered subjects, objects, and indirect objects; the thematic roles played by female-and male-gendered NPs; and the kinds of states, activities, and attributes predicated of female and male participants in the sentences. Second, we present the results of a survey of gender distribution and stereotyping in example sentences from ten other syntax textbooks, published between 1969 and 1994. Results from both studies indicate that little has changed over the past twenty-five years: all but two of the authors favor male-gendered NPs as subjects and agents, and regularly stereotype males and females.1 Problems like these were officially recognized in the scholarly writings of linguistics when, in 1992, the LSA adopted guidelines for nonsexist usage. These guidelines explic-* We are grateful to the editors of Cultural Performances: Proceedings of the Third Berkeley Women and , and the several different audiences to whom we have read versions of this work for their many suggestions. M. Macaulay would like to thank Edie Thornton for teaching the women's studies class that she sat in on; many of the ideas in this paper were influenced by things she lea...
The Algonquian prominence hierarchy, usually characterized as 2 > 1 > 3, is often cited as a counterexample to claims that 1st person outranks 2nd universally. Data from five Algonquian languages show that rankings 1 > 2, 2 > 1, and 1 = 2 are actually found, depending on the affix position and language, leading to the conclusion that the hierarchy should be SAP > 3. We also find two distinct models of hierarchy: a scalar model upon which a transition between paradigms or categories of affixes is made at various points by different languages, and a relational model which requires an evaluation of the relative ranking of two arguments on a scale, with the conclusion leading to a choice between markers within a single paradigm.
This article examines theoretical and typological characterizations of evidentials. Based on the literature and newly analyzed data from Karuk (a Native American language of California), we argue that two properties are criterial: (i) marking source of evidence and (ii) membership in grammatical systems. Other properties vary crosslinguistically: presence of epistemic, illocutionary, or mirative meaning; speaker deixis; obligatoriness; complementarity of meaning with other items; and truth-conditionality. The values of these variable properties cannot be assumed but must be empirically determined for individual items and languages. Our characterization can serve as a guide for research, examination of the relationship between evidentiality and other categories, and typological work on evidentials.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.