People find their way through cluttered environments with ease and without injury. How do they do it? Two approaches to wayfmding are considered: Differential motion parallax (DMP) is a retinal motion invariant of near and far objects moving against fixation; the information in optical flow (IOF) is a radial pattern of vectors, relying on decomposition of retinal flow. Evidence is presented that DMP guides wayfinding during natural gait, accounting for errors as well as correct responses. Evidence against IOF is also presented, and a space-time aliasing artifact that can contaminate IOF displays is explored. Finally, DMP and IOF are separated, showing they can yield different results in different environments. Thus, it is concluded that (a) DMP and IOF are different, (b) DMP and not IOF is used for wayfinding, (c) moving observers do not usually decompose retinal flow, and (d) optical flow may be a mathematical fiction with no psychological reality.One of the most compelling of all visual phenomena occurs when one hurtles through the environment. The resulting radial streams of motion by surrounding objects, sometimes called optical flow, have captured the imagination of writers, artists, and cinematographers, as well as psychologists, neuroscientists, and computer scientists. This global motion was probably first noticed by the general populace in the mid19th century, but then only in industrializing nations and with the widespread use of railroads.The reasons for the relatively recent focus on optical flow are probably threefold: On a train one could, for the first time, travel (a) at velocities greater than about 4 eye heights/ s for a sustained period of time; 1 (b) on a relatively smooth roadbed that eliminated the bouncing caused by one's own footfall or that of a horse, or by the jostling of a coach; and (c) with free time to look about, unfettered by the demands of guiding one's course through the environment. Naturally, railway travel offered much more than noticeable optical flow. Not all of it was good. Indeed, around 1860 The Lancet published a series of articles on putative ill effects of rail travel This research was supported by National Science Foundation Grant BNS-8818971 to James E. Cutting.Experiment 1 was reported at the 30th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Atlanta, November 1989; parts of Experiments 2 and 3 at the Society's 31st Annual Meeting, New Orleans, November 1990; and parts of Experiments 7 and 8 at its 32nd Annual Meeting, San Francisco, November 1991. Experiments 1, 2, 4, 5, and 6 were also reported at a perception-action workshop at Storrs, Connecticut, in July 1990.We thank Laurence G. Kaplan for initial and sustaining help in programming the Personal Iris Workstation; David Sabean for discussions of 19th-century perception; Gretchen Van de Walle, Jack Loomis, and several anonymous reviewers for their comments; and Julian Hochberg and Nan Karwan for various discussions of this project over the past 6 yearsCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to J...
When moving through cluttered environments we use different forms of the same source of information to avoid stationary and moving objects. A stationary obstacle can be avoided by looking at it, registering the differential parallactic displacements on the retina around it during pursuit fixation, and then acting on that information. Such information also specifies one's general heading. A moving obstacle can be avoided by looking at it, registering the displacements reflecting constancy or change in one's gaze-movement angle, and then acting on that information. Such information, however, does not generally specify one's heading. Passing in front of a moving object entails retrograde motion of objects in the deep background; collisions entail the lamellar pattern of optical flow; and passing behind entails more nearly uniform flow against one's direction of motion. Accuracy in the laboratory compares favorably with that of real-world necessities.We and other animals move through cluttered environments many times each day, often at considerable speed. Most objects in these environments are stationary and need to be avoided if we are to get safely from one place to another. Some objects also move, and these too often must be avoided. 1 Such acts of avoidance are of obvious and considerable importance; to fail to execute them with reasonable accuracy is to risk our daily well-being as well as that of others. What visual information subserves these acts, particularly for mobile-eyed creatures like ourselves?Psychological research on collisions and how to avoid them began with Gibson and Crooks (1938; see also Gibson, 1961). This research then progressed in several directions. One line has focused on driver behavior and automobile safety (e.g., Caird &
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.