The seasonal decline in clutch size has been explained as being due to either: (1) a constraining effect of female condition on both laying date and clutch size; or (2) a seasonal decline in the prospects of chick recruitment, leading to a strategic decrease in clutch size with laying date. In an effort to shed light on this area of disagreement, we analyzed the physiological condition (as measured by body mass, breast muscle thickness, and fat stores) and body size (as measured by wing and head lengths) of 184 female Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) nesting near Ithaca, New York in 1993, and 1995, and related their condition to their laying date and clutch size. Through multiple regressions, we found that female age and wing length were the only significant predictors of laying date and that female age and laying date were the only significant predictors of clutch size. Thus, when the effects of laying date were held constant, there was no detectable effect of condition on clutch size in this species. This result is inconsistent with the constrainingcondition hypothesis, and it suggests that the seasonal decline of clutch size in Tree Swallows is most appropriately seen as a strategic adjustment by the female to varying prospects for her offspring. The lack of evolution in laying date in at least the Tree Swallow remains a paradox.
Summary 1.To minimize study-area artefacts in a study of natal dispersal distances in tree swallows, we intensively banded nestlings and adults in nest-boxes in upstate New York and recruited and trained 70 volunteer banders within a 400 km radius of Ithaca. We banded 26 567 nestlings in the years 1985 through 1998, and captured 4774 adults at the nest, 630 of which had been banded as nestlings and were recaptured the year after fledging. 2. To correct for spatial variation in capture intensity, we resampled the distribution of all boxes where an adult capture was made under uniform, exponential and Cauchy null hypotheses. Compared to the null distributions, the frequency of observed dispersal events was significantly higher at 0 -10 km and lower for all larger dispersal distances. The Cauchy distribution came closest to approximating the observed dispersal distance distribution. 3. Dispersal distances were sensitive to the distribution of nesting sites, and all measurements of dispersal distance distributions must be seen as being habitat-specific. We could detect no effect of dispersal distance on the subsequent timing of breeding and no effect of the timing of fledging on dispersal distance. The sexual differences in raw mean dispersal distances (8·38 km for females, 2·44 km for males) are similar to those reported for other species. 4. While it is tempting to conclude that studies in smaller areas have not missed a great deal, even a study area of 10 km extent (substantially larger than most) would have missed 11% of the dispersing birds detected and 95% of the range of distances recorded. Despite this sizeable component that would have been missed with a smaller study area, the relatively low frequencies of long-distance dispersal overall reinforce the conclusion that tree swallows, and probably most other migratory passerines, generally disperse much less far from their natal sites than the distances of their annual migrations might lead one to expect.
To study the patterns and determinants of philopatry and breeding dispersal in the Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor) we analyzed the records of 356 males and 1459 females captured in more than one breeding year around Ithaca, New York. Of these captures, only 4% of male and 14% of female breeders dispersed to a new site for breeding. With our combination of intensive study areas in Tompkins County, New York, and the efforts of volunteer banders throughout New York and surrounding states, we could have detected dispersal in excess of 400 km from the initial breeding site. Randomization tests revealed, however, that breeders dispersed much shorter distances than they could have been detected. Detailed analyses of recaptures in Tompkins County showed that over a 22-km range of distances, the chances of dispersal to a new breeding site declined with the distance from the original breeding site. Females that failed to fledge any offspring were much more likely to disperse than females that reproduced successfully, and the probability of dispersal declined gradually with female age. The spatial scale in which swallows gather and process information appears to be much larger than for passerines that defend all-purpose territories.Dispersión Reproductiva y Filopatría en Tachycineta bicolorResumen. Para estudiar los patrones y los determinantes de la filopatría y la dispersión reproductiva en Tachycineta bicolor analizamos los registros de 356 machos y 1459 hembras capturados en más de un año reproductivo en los alrededores de Ithaca, New York. De estas capturas, sólo el 4% de los machos y el 14% de las hembras reproductivas se dispersaron a un nuevo sitio de cría. Con nuestra combinación de áreas de estudio intensivas en el Condado de Tompkins, New York, y los esfuerzos de colaboradores voluntarios que anillaron aves a lo largo de New York y los estados circundantes, pudimos haber detectado eventos de dispersión a más de 400 km desde el sitio de cría inicial. Exámenes aleatorizados revelaron, sin embargo, que las aves reproductivas se dispersaron a distancias mucho más cortas que las que se podrían haber detectado. Análisis detallados de recapturas en el Condado de Tompkins mostraron que en un rango de distancias de 22 km, las probabilidades de dispersión a un nuevo sitio de cría disminuyeron con la distancia desde el sitio de cría original. Las hembras que fracasaron en la cría de pichones presentaron una probabilidad de dispersarse mucho mayor que las hembras que se reprodujeron exitosamente, y la probabilidad de dispersión disminuyó gradualmente con la edad de la hembra. La escala espacial a la que T. bicolor recoge y procesa información parece ser mucho más grande que la de aves paserinas que defienden territorios de uso múltiple.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2025 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.