1991
DOI: 10.1016/0361-9230(91)90133-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Brain mechanisms of taste aversion learning in the rat

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

6
68
4

Year Published

1994
1994
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 129 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
6
68
4
Order By: Relevance
“…The overall conclusions are still debated, and the results appear to depend on the type of lesion (whether surgical ablation, electrolytic, cytotoxic, or metabolic lesion), its location within the amygdala, its duration (permanent vs. transient), and its timing relative to training and testing. Although some investigations revealed no significant effects of certain amygdala lesions on CTA (Kemble et al 1979;Hatfield et al 1992;Galaverna et al 1993), in most studies, amygdala lesions, especially of the basolateral nucleus (Fitzgerald and Burton 1983;Simbayi et al 1986;Yamamoto and Fujimoto 1991;Roldan and Bures 1994;Yamamoto et al 1995), but also including the CeA (Lasiter andGlanzman 1982 1985;Roldan and Bures 1994), disrupted acquisition of CTA, its retrieval, or both. Roldan and Bures (1994) suggested that amygdaloid nuclei subserve CTA in the intact brain, but that their permanent absence can be compensated for by other brain centers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The overall conclusions are still debated, and the results appear to depend on the type of lesion (whether surgical ablation, electrolytic, cytotoxic, or metabolic lesion), its location within the amygdala, its duration (permanent vs. transient), and its timing relative to training and testing. Although some investigations revealed no significant effects of certain amygdala lesions on CTA (Kemble et al 1979;Hatfield et al 1992;Galaverna et al 1993), in most studies, amygdala lesions, especially of the basolateral nucleus (Fitzgerald and Burton 1983;Simbayi et al 1986;Yamamoto and Fujimoto 1991;Roldan and Bures 1994;Yamamoto et al 1995), but also including the CeA (Lasiter andGlanzman 1982 1985;Roldan and Bures 1994), disrupted acquisition of CTA, its retrieval, or both. Roldan and Bures (1994) suggested that amygdaloid nuclei subserve CTA in the intact brain, but that their permanent absence can be compensated for by other brain centers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Early research favored a role for the GT in CTA acquisition (e.g., Lasiter, 1985;Loullis, Wayner, & Jolicoeur, 1978;Yamamoto, 1993;Yamamoto & Fujimoto, 1991;Yamamoto et al 1995). However, more recent research, using electrolytic lesions that caused minimal damage beyond the boundaries of the GT, finds no evidence that discrete GT lesions disrupt first-order CTA (e.g., Flynn, Grill, Schulkin, & Norgren, 1991;Grigson, Lyuboslavsky, & Tanase, 2000;Reilly & Pritchard, 1996).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several lines of evidence indicate that in adult rats, the amygdala plays a crucial role in both fear conditioning and odor-LiCl conditioning (Bermudez-Rattoni et al 1986;Ferry and Di Scala 1997;LeDoux 2000;Gale et al 2004;Touzani and Sclafani 2005). It should be noted that the basolateral complex of the amygdala is essential for odormalaise learning, although results are inconsistent for tastemalaise learning (Nachman and Ashe 1974;Burt and Smotherman 1980;Bermudez-Rattoni et al 1986;Dunn and Everitt 1988;Yamamoto and Fujimoto 1991;Kesner et al 1992;Sakai and Yamamoto 1999;Bahar et al 2004;Touzani and Sclafani 2005;Wilkins and Bernstein 2006).…”
Section: Amygdala Basolateral Complexmentioning
confidence: 99%