2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.04.015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive differences in horses performing locomotor versus oral stereotypic behaviour

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This increased switching in the crib-biting group provides us with useful evidence regarding the aetiology of equine oral stereotypy and is in agreement with the finding of Roberts et al (2015) who found that a group of 8 crib biting horses performed significantly more switching behaviours that 8 normal horses. These results lend weight to the notion of increased transmission of the neurotransmitter dopamine (an important pleasure neurotransmitter) in this group of horses.…”
Section: Dietsupporting
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This increased switching in the crib-biting group provides us with useful evidence regarding the aetiology of equine oral stereotypy and is in agreement with the finding of Roberts et al (2015) who found that a group of 8 crib biting horses performed significantly more switching behaviours that 8 normal horses. These results lend weight to the notion of increased transmission of the neurotransmitter dopamine (an important pleasure neurotransmitter) in this group of horses.…”
Section: Dietsupporting
confidence: 78%
“…This is supported by McGreevy et al (1995) who reported a positive association between stereotypic behaviour in Thoroughbreds when fed less than 6.8 kg of fibre/day. Furthermore, the feeding of concentrates after weaning (a feeding regimen often associated with low forage provision) led to a 4-fold increase in the initial development of crib-biting (Waters et al, 2002), and has also been shown to elicit post-development increases in crib-biting activity (Gillham et al, 1994 feed induced elevations have been attributed to neurotransmission of dopamine (Roberts et al, 2015), and more complex fluctuations in neuro-active molecules such as leptin and Ghrelin (Hemmann et al, 2013). Forage on the other hand, elicits a depression in crib-biting intensity (Hemmings and Hale, 2013) although the precise schedule of forage provision does not impact upon locomotor activity (stereotypic or otherwise) in a 24 h period (Piccione et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research has demonstrated that individuals exhibiting stereotypic behaviours tend to have stunted learning and problem solving abilities and that the type of stereotypy a horse engages in (such as weaving or cribbing) may influence their behaviour on certain cognitive tasks (Kirsty et al, 2015). Hausberger et al (2007) found that horses with stereotypic behaviours, such as weaving, cribbing, head shaking, and tongue play, tend to take longer to learn and complete basic tasks, such as pushing open a chest for a food reward, when compared to non-stereotypic horses.…”
Section: The Relationship Between Horse Cognition Training and Welfarementioning
confidence: 99%
“…data). Thus, all studies (Hemmings et al, 2007;Hausberger et al, 2007a;Kirsty et al, 2015;Parker et al, 2008Parker et al, , 2009 until now have demonstrated learning impairments in stereotypic horses. Only one recent study (Briefer Freymond et al, 2019) seems to contradict this finding: using a reversal learning task, the authors did not find any difference in learning performance between crib-biters and nonstereotypic horses (as identified by owners).…”
Section: Animal-based Indicators and Effects Of A Compromised Welfare State On Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%