2009
DOI: 10.1167/9.4.16
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Color constancy improves for real 3D objects

Abstract: In this study human color constancy was tested for two-dimensional (2D) and three-dimensional (3D) setups with real objects and lights. Four different illuminant changes, a natural selection task and a wide choice of target colors were used. We found that color constancy was better when the target color was learned as a 3D object in a cue-rich 3D scene than in a 2D setup. This improvement was independent of the target color and the illuminant change. We were not able to find any evidence that frequently experi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
48
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
1
48
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The size of the illumination context also has a large effect on color constancy, at least for simple displays (Hansen et al, 2007;Murray, Daugirdiene, Vaitkevicius, Kulikowski, & Stanikunas, 2006;Rinner & Gegenfurtner, 2000). Moreover, cues from 3D scene geometry (Bloj, Kersten, & Hurlbert, 1999;Hedrich, Bloj, & Ruppertsberg, 2009), highlights (Snyder, Doerschner, & Maloney, 2005;Yang & Shevell, 2003), and stereo disparity (Werner, 2006;Yang & Shevell, 2002) appear to be used in estimating surface reflectance and illumination.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The size of the illumination context also has a large effect on color constancy, at least for simple displays (Hansen et al, 2007;Murray, Daugirdiene, Vaitkevicius, Kulikowski, & Stanikunas, 2006;Rinner & Gegenfurtner, 2000). Moreover, cues from 3D scene geometry (Bloj, Kersten, & Hurlbert, 1999;Hedrich, Bloj, & Ruppertsberg, 2009), highlights (Snyder, Doerschner, & Maloney, 2005;Yang & Shevell, 2003), and stereo disparity (Werner, 2006;Yang & Shevell, 2002) appear to be used in estimating surface reflectance and illumination.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Overall, photographs of natural scenes seen in color are recognized quicker and remembered better than when grayscale is used [19,20] and even in the case of simple lights, chromatic components are better remembered than brightness ones [21]. Our ability to remember colors has been put to practical use in applications such as color quality metrics for solid-state light sources [22] and to enhance digital images [23] and plays a significant role in color constancy [24][25][26][27].Given this important role of color in visual memory, large biases would be somewhat surprising. However, our results do confirm previous studies in finding an increase in saturation in the memory construct compared to direct viewing.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Delahunt and Brainard (2004b) could not report a clear advantage of daylight illuminant changes over other illuminant changes. Daugirdiene, Murray, Vaitkevicius, and Kulikowski (2006) also compared color constancy levels for on-and off-blackbody locus illuminants and did not find superior constancy for the on-blackbody locus illuminants, in line with Hedrich, Bloj, and Ruppertsberg (2009). However, a recent report (Crichton, Pearce, Mackiewicz, Finlayson, & Hurlbert, 2012) measured color constancy for a scene with real objects under a broad range of illuminations, both on and off the daylight locus.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%