1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03211419
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conceptual priming in a generative problem-solving task

Abstract: Three experiments explored how participants solved a very open-ended generative problem-solving task. Previous research has shown that when participants are shown examples, novel creations will tend to conform to features shared across those examples (Smith, Ward, & Schumacher, 1993); We made the shared features of the examples conceptually related to one another. Wefound that when the features were related to the concept of hostility, participants' creations contained hostile features that were not part of an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
32
0
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
3
32
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This enables them to think outside the box. Priming can, in fact, reduce creativity, as the participants tend to be preoccupied with already-known solutions (Dahl & Moreau, 2002;Marsh, Bink, & Hicks, 1999).…”
Section: An Employee-driven Methods Facilitating Collective Creativitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This enables them to think outside the box. Priming can, in fact, reduce creativity, as the participants tend to be preoccupied with already-known solutions (Dahl & Moreau, 2002;Marsh, Bink, & Hicks, 1999).…”
Section: An Employee-driven Methods Facilitating Collective Creativitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The conformity to given examples did not decrease when participants were explicitly instructed to create ideas that were very diVerent from the provided examples, even though they were able to list the features they were asked to avoid (Marsh, Ward, & Landau, 1999;Smith, Ward, & Schumacher, 1993). Most likely the plagiarism occurs because examples are highly accessible during idea generation and this activated knowledge impacts on the generated ideas without awareness (Marsh, Bink, & Hicks, 1999) and thus beyond intentional control.…”
Section: Priming Creativity and The Mindset "Think Diverent"mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Prime examples include such phenomena as functional fixedness, which involves restricting the uses of objects to well-known functions (Maier, 1931), and mental set, which involves situationally induced obstacles to problem solving (Luchins, 1942). Furthermore, it has been found that providing (Dahl & Moreau, 2002;Jaarsveld & van Leeuwen, 2005;Jansson & Smith, 1991;Marsh, Bink, & Hicks, 1999;Marsh, Ward, & Landau, 1999;Ward, 1994) or retrieving (Ward, 1994) existing examples may inhibit generative creative processes and may lead to a higher proportion of property transfers from the examples into the subject's own work (e.g., Marsh, Landau, & Hicks, 1996), even when the subject is explicitly instructed to avoid such transfer (e.g., Smith, Ward, & Schumacher, 1993). Source monitoring of this property transfer is especially poor in generative tasks (e.g., Marsh, Landau, & Hicks, 1997), which originally led to the label unconscious plagiarism, or cryptomnesia (Brown & Murphy, 1989;Marsh & Bower, 1993;Marsh & Landau, 1995;.…”
Section: Preinventive Structures and Analogymentioning
confidence: 99%