2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Copy me or copy you? The effect of prior experience on social learning

Abstract: The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-pro t purposes provided that:• a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.Please consult the full DRO policy… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

8
73
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
8
73
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Factors such as functional fixedness (German & Defeyter, 2000), explicit instruction (Bonawitz et al, 2011), prior social information (Wood et al, 2013a), and task structure (Cutting, Apperly, Chappell, & Beck, 2014) likely constrain it. Innovation can be delineated in terms of arising from asocial learning (innovation by independent invention) or a combination of asocial and social learning (innovation by modification; Carr, Kendal, & Flynn, under revision).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Factors such as functional fixedness (German & Defeyter, 2000), explicit instruction (Bonawitz et al, 2011), prior social information (Wood et al, 2013a), and task structure (Cutting, Apperly, Chappell, & Beck, 2014) likely constrain it. Innovation can be delineated in terms of arising from asocial learning (innovation by independent invention) or a combination of asocial and social learning (innovation by modification; Carr, Kendal, & Flynn, under revision).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…However, children are not blind to the quality of information they observe. By altering the frequency and fidelity with which they imitate, in line with the perceived goal of a demonstration (Bekkering, Wohlschläger, & Gattis, 2000;Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello, 2005), model reliability and intentionality (Birch, Vauthier, & Bloom, 2008;Carpenter, Akhtar, & Tomasello, 1998), task difficulty and prior experience (Gardiner, Bjorklund, Greif, & Gray, 2012;Pinkham & Jaswal, 2011;Williamson & Meltzoff, 2011;Wood et al, 2013a), children display rationality and flexibility in their social learning (Koenig & Sabbagh, 2013;Mills, 2013;Over & Carpenter, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Context also affects biases toward copying models belonging to a particular group, 290 such as an age group. One-and two-year-olds generally imitate spontaneous behaviour 291 exhibited by adults more than peers, but the context affects this replication, such that early childhood; four-to five-year-old children imitate adults more than children and same 297 age peers when an action is novel, but copy a peer over an older child and adult when the action is not novel (Zmyj et al, 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The second related issue is that young children may struggle to differentiate the subtle (Wood, Kendal & Flynn, 2013a). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%