Publisher's copyright statement: NOTICE: this is the author's version of a work that was accepted for publication in Developmental Review. Changes resulting from the publishing process, such as peer review, editing, corrections, structural formatting, and other quality control mechanisms may not be reected in this document. Changes may have been made to this work since it was submitted for publication. A denitive version was subsequently published in Developmental Review, 33, 4, December 2013, 10.1016/j.dr.2013.002.
Additional information:Use policyThe full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that:• a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.Please consult the full DRO policy for further details. to the frequency at which they are practiced (Rendell et al., 2011).
15The adaptive value of model-based biases have been investigated in disciplines such 16 as evolutionary biology, anthropology and non-developmental domains of psychology.
17Model-based biases have been described as "who" biases (Laland, 2004) In this section we make five propositions regarding the adaptive value of model-based biases.
54To begin, we examine children"s biases towards models whose behaviour indicates their 55 desire to transfer information, namely children"s receptiveness to pedagogical cueing. 2 for instances where this does not happen). indicating that children may select information from those more genetically related to them.
140Familiarity is a confound to this interpretation, yet familiarity itself can be a marker of in- the history of the stranger is unknown and, therefore, the information they provide may not 150 be relevant for the child"s particular environment. they stated that they "liked" rather than peers they stated that they did "not like". Likewise,
200McGuigan (2013) found that the higher the status of the model (the child"s head teacher 201 versus a known researcher) the greater the number of irrelevant actions were copied.
202Whether a model is observed by others may, in itself, be a marker of prestige; four-203 year-old children use bystanders" silent reactions to models such that a model who was
204"endorsed" by bystanders through nods and smiles was copied more for labelling novel biased copying from prestigious models is, in some circumstances, limited to behaviour