2015
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4517-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cross-modal perceptual load: the impact of modality and individual differences

Abstract: Visual distractor processing tends to be more pronounced when the perceptual load (PL) of a task is low compared to when it is high [perpetual load theory (PLT); Lavie in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(3):451-468, 1995]. While PLT is well established in the visual domain, application to cross-modal processing has produced mixed results, and the current study was designed in an attempt to improve previous methodologies. First, we assessed PLT using response competition, a typical metric from the uni-modal… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
15
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
2
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There is evidence that the perceptual load model is modality specific; for example, a PET study found that auditory perceptual load did not reduce activity in motion‐related visual areas in response to an irrelevant visual distractor (Rees, Frith, & Lavie, ). Other perceptual load studies have since supported this study, finding no evidence of cross‐modal load effects (Tellinghuisen & Nowak, ; Vroomen, Driver, & De Gelder, ; Jacoby, Hall, & Mattingley, ; Sandhu & Dyson, ). However, there is also conflicting perceptual load research that supports a supramodal view of attention (Sinnett, Costa, & Soto‐Faraco, ; Raveh & Lavie, ; Kreitz, Furley, Simons, & Memmert, ); for example, a recent MEG study found that suppression of auditory evoked activity for an irrelevant tone when participants were performing a high visual load task (Molloy, Griffiths, Chait, & Lavie, ).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 62%
“…There is evidence that the perceptual load model is modality specific; for example, a PET study found that auditory perceptual load did not reduce activity in motion‐related visual areas in response to an irrelevant visual distractor (Rees, Frith, & Lavie, ). Other perceptual load studies have since supported this study, finding no evidence of cross‐modal load effects (Tellinghuisen & Nowak, ; Vroomen, Driver, & De Gelder, ; Jacoby, Hall, & Mattingley, ; Sandhu & Dyson, ). However, there is also conflicting perceptual load research that supports a supramodal view of attention (Sinnett, Costa, & Soto‐Faraco, ; Raveh & Lavie, ; Kreitz, Furley, Simons, & Memmert, ); for example, a recent MEG study found that suppression of auditory evoked activity for an irrelevant tone when participants were performing a high visual load task (Molloy, Griffiths, Chait, & Lavie, ).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 62%
“…), and how it relates to general concerns about working memory and attention (see also discussion in Kutas & Federmeier 26 regarding the N400 and attention). Our findings also recommend connecting this type of inquiry with work on unimodal versus cross-modal attention 72 and multi-sensory integration more broadly 73 . Participants' scores ranged from 1.00 to 4.86 on the Innovative Musical Aptitude subscale of the Brief Music Experience Questionnaire 76 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…This allows for limited additional capacity for the perception of task-irrelevant stimuli, resulting in diminished processing of events unrelated to task goals [1, 25]. Most of the work in support of this theory has involved unimodal, not cross-modal, paradigms [80]. Our findings raise the possibility that in cross-modal tasks, increased cognitive load of the primary visual task may influence distracter processing in a manner that is similar to increased perceptual load in unimodal tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%