2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.trf.2014.08.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deep in thought while driving: An EEG study on drivers’ cognitive distraction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
39
0
3

Year Published

2018
2018
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 93 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
1
39
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The findings represent that the force-feedback could correlate with higher ability in decision making and ultimately increase the capability of controlling the vehicle properly at the time of hazard encounter. It was initially expected to get the consistent results with Almahasneh et al (2014) findings, however, the topographical map result ( Figure 3) indicates that the difference between baseline and both cases is caused by more activity in corresponding brain region of the right frontal hemisphere near reaching the curves. Since most of cognitive activities occur at the frontal lobe (Lin et al, 2011) the findings are aligned with the role of frontal lobe in decision making and attention (Burgess, Alderman, Volle, Benoit, & Gilbert, 2009).…”
Section: Analysis Of Cognitive Statessupporting
confidence: 54%
“…The findings represent that the force-feedback could correlate with higher ability in decision making and ultimately increase the capability of controlling the vehicle properly at the time of hazard encounter. It was initially expected to get the consistent results with Almahasneh et al (2014) findings, however, the topographical map result ( Figure 3) indicates that the difference between baseline and both cases is caused by more activity in corresponding brain region of the right frontal hemisphere near reaching the curves. Since most of cognitive activities occur at the frontal lobe (Lin et al, 2011) the findings are aligned with the role of frontal lobe in decision making and attention (Burgess, Alderman, Volle, Benoit, & Gilbert, 2009).…”
Section: Analysis Of Cognitive Statessupporting
confidence: 54%
“…The driving duration was considered prolonged when it took at least 120 minutes [20], although other researchers set 150-180 minutes as the prolonged driving threshold [16,21]. The route chosen for this simulation was a loop track comprising of freeways and city streets, as provided by the driving simulation software used, City Car Driving Simulator v.1.4, which is commonly used in driving simulation studies (e.g., Almahasneh, et al [22]). The traffic density was set at 80% and the suggested driving speed was in the range of 40 to 60 km/hour.…”
Section: Tasks Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Numerous studies conducted via driving simulators, test tracks, and naturalistic driving research have sought to quantify and clarify the risks of distracted driving due to visual and manual secondary tasks (e.g., Dingus et al, 2016;Fitch et al, 2013;Guo et al, 2017;Klauer et al, 2014;Klauer, Dingus, Neale, Sudweeks, & Ramsey, 2006;Olson, Hanowski, Hickman, & Bocanegra, 2009;Victor et al, 2015). However, existing research into cognitive disengagement relative to automobile use and risk (e.g., Almahasneh, Chooi, Kamel, & Malik, 2014;Angell, Perez, & Soccolich, 2014;Briggs, Hole, & Land, 2011;Owens et al, 2018;Strayer et al, 2013;Strayer et al, 2014;Wang, Reimer, Dobres, & Mehler, 2014) appears to be less conclusive. Current discrepancies are likely due to different data sources and study methodologies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%