1986
DOI: 10.3758/bf03208189
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does perceptual adaptation to telestereoscopically enhanced depth depend on the recalibration of binocular disparity?

Abstract: Apparent depth is significantly reduced after viewing a rotating three-dimensional form through a system of mirrors which enhances binocular disparity by optically increasing the separation between the eyes. The change in depth perception has previously been interpreted in terms of the adaptive recalibration of binocular disparity by kinetic sources of depth information. The present experiments showed that the same result is still obtained when either disparity or kinetic depth is absent during the exposure in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Wallach et al (1963) and Epstein and Morgan-Paap (1974) both reported disparity recalibration resulting from brief viewing of stimuli in which disparity conflicted with other visual cues. However, these observed perceptual aftereffects are largely attributable to changes in tonic vergence (Fisher and Ebenholtz, 1986;Fisher and Ciuffreda, 1990) and "normalization," or "satiation" slant aftereffects (e.g., Köhler and Emery, 1947). The present study provides a clear demonstration of visual recalibration, driven by another visual cue, and moreover offers a description of the conditions under which this recalibration is optimized.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Wallach et al (1963) and Epstein and Morgan-Paap (1974) both reported disparity recalibration resulting from brief viewing of stimuli in which disparity conflicted with other visual cues. However, these observed perceptual aftereffects are largely attributable to changes in tonic vergence (Fisher and Ebenholtz, 1986;Fisher and Ciuffreda, 1990) and "normalization," or "satiation" slant aftereffects (e.g., Köhler and Emery, 1947). The present study provides a clear demonstration of visual recalibration, driven by another visual cue, and moreover offers a description of the conditions under which this recalibration is optimized.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…There is a small literature on indirect effects of accommodation on perception. Fisher and Ebenholtz (1986), Mon-Williams and Tresilian (2000), and Wallach and Norris (1963) observed an influence of accommodation on depth interpretation (for a negative result, see Ritter, 1977). Heinemann, Tulving, andNachmias (1959) andvon Holst (1973) observed an influence of accommodation on perceived size.…”
Section: Direct Versus Indirect Influence Of Focus Cues On Perceived mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One possibility is that vergence information provides an approximate calibration for stereo depth [9,10]. Structure from motion provides accurate shape information, assuming object rigidity, but the near/far relations between the object and the observer are not specified [5].…”
Section: B Other Depth Cues and Conflictsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This hypothesis is countered by evidence that stereopsis affects the degree of apparent depth in a SFM display [8]. Overall, it seems clear that depth derived solely from disparity information is inaccurate [9], and can be recalibrated dynamically [10,11,12], although ordinal information provided from stereo can be extremely precise. This suggests that dynamically changing stereo depth parameters is probably viable and certainly worth studying.…”
Section: B Other Depth Cues and Conflictsmentioning
confidence: 99%