1988
DOI: 10.1177/154193128803200213
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effectiveness of Three-Dimensional Auditory Directional Cues

Abstract: Natural aural directional cueing in the cockpit should relieve the demands placed on the visual modality, reduce display clutter and alleviate cognitive attention needed to process and extract meaning from coded formats. This experiment compared the effectiveness of three-dimensional (3-D) auditory cues to conventional visual and auditory methods of directing visual attention to peripheral targets. Five directional cues were evaluated: visual symbol, coded aural tone, speech cue, 3-D tone (white noise appearin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The finding that a 3-D auditory display led to improved user performance is in general agreement with the results of previous investigators (Arrabito et al, 2001;Begault, 1993;Begault & Erbe, 1994;Bronkhorst et al, 1996;Calhoun et al, 1987Calhoun et al, , 1988. For example, in a visual search task Begault (1993) evaluated a "head-up" spatial auditory air traffic collision avoidance system (TCAS) in a flight simulator.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The finding that a 3-D auditory display led to improved user performance is in general agreement with the results of previous investigators (Arrabito et al, 2001;Begault, 1993;Begault & Erbe, 1994;Bronkhorst et al, 1996;Calhoun et al, 1987Calhoun et al, , 1988. For example, in a visual search task Begault (1993) evaluated a "head-up" spatial auditory air traffic collision avoidance system (TCAS) in a flight simulator.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…The headphone presentation of a sound that is convolved with the time domain representation of a head-related transfer function (HRTF) -the digital filter used for the spatial synthesis -is perceived by the listener to emanate from outside his or her head at the location for which the HRTF was measured (Bronkhorst, 1995;Møller, Sørensen, Hammershøi, & Jensen, 1995;Wightman & Kistler, 1989b). This technology has led to improved operator performance in a variety of applications, such as auditory alarms in aircraft cockpits (Calhoun, Janson, & Valencia, 1988;Calhoun, Valencia, & Furness, 1987), visual search tasks (Begault,1993;Bronkhorst, Veltman, & van Breda, 1996), release from masking (Doll et al,1992), and speech intelligibility (Arrabito, McFadden, & Crabtree, 2001;Begault & Erbe, 1994). Although Doll et al (1992) found that the largest MLD was obtained when the signal was spatially separated from the masker in a simulated sonar task, the efficacy of a 3-D auditory display cannot be assessed for the problem under consideration because the sounds were not presented in virtual auditory space.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Apart from the fact that reaction times to auditory stimuli are typically faster than to visual stimuli (e.g., Galton, 1899; Sanders, 1998; Woodworth & Schlosberg, 1954), sound is often the most effective medium for conveying information. Auditory signals can be attention-grabbing regardless of the operator's visual focus, accommodating operator mobility, allowing events both inside and outside the operator's field of view to be monitored (Burt, Bartolome, Burdette, & Comstock, 1995; Calhoun, Janson, & Valencia, 1988; Doll & Folds, 1986; Edworthy & Adams, 1996; Edworthy & Stanton, 1995; Gaver, 1989; Haas & Casali, 1995; McKinley & Ericson, 1997; Stokes & Wickens, 1988; Szalma et al, 2004). Furthermore, some dynamically changing events (e.g., approaching automobiles) are more readily represented in auditory than in visual displays (Gaver, 1989, 1993).…”
Section: Review Of Theory Research and Applicationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Auditory icons or caricatures of everyday sounds (Belz, Robinson, & Casali, 1999;Calhoun, Janson, & Valencia, 1988;Gaver, 1989Gaver, , 1993Graham, 1999;Ho & Spence, 2005) circumvent a number of the problems associated with abstract alarms as they involve a relation between a signal and referent -for example, the sound of coughing to signal dangerous levels of gas. Auditory icons can be short, are not easily masked, and are generally recognizable and distinguishable (Keller & Cough Once for Danger: Icons Versus Abstract Warnings as Informative Alerts in Civil Aviation Nathan C. Perry, Catherine J. Stevens, Mark W. Wiggins, and Clare E. Howell, University of Western Sydney-Bankstown, South Penrith, Australia…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%