1981
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.7.2.441
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Estimates of utility function parameters from signal detection experiments.

Abstract: A theory is developed to make it possible to estimate the slope parameter of a presumed power law utility function from an analysis of data from signal detection experiments. The theory can be applied to detection data analyzed either according to constant likelihood ratio models or according to linear learning models of bias. Experiments to test the consequences of the theory are described. A linear learning model is found to give the best account of the data. The utility function obtained from this model is … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Instead of setting the criterion on the basis of some monotonic transformation of the likelihood ratio, as suggested by SDT, the Wolfe et al (2007) model suggests that users somehow keep a mental tally of errors that is then used to place the criterion. This model does an admirable job of explaining the consistent empirical finding (e.g., Kornbrot, Donnelly, & Galanter, 1981;Thomas, 1975) that observers fail to shift their decision criterion as far as detection theory says that they should in low-prevalence conditions. However, given that observers also exhibit similar numbers of misses and false alarms in the absence of feedback (Wolfe et al, 2007), for the theory to hold, observers must provide their own response feedback internally.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead of setting the criterion on the basis of some monotonic transformation of the likelihood ratio, as suggested by SDT, the Wolfe et al (2007) model suggests that users somehow keep a mental tally of errors that is then used to place the criterion. This model does an admirable job of explaining the consistent empirical finding (e.g., Kornbrot, Donnelly, & Galanter, 1981;Thomas, 1975) that observers fail to shift their decision criterion as far as detection theory says that they should in low-prevalence conditions. However, given that observers also exhibit similar numbers of misses and false alarms in the absence of feedback (Wolfe et al, 2007), for the theory to hold, observers must provide their own response feedback internally.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Assuming a power-law utility function, Galanter estimated the power to be 0.43. Kornbrot, Donnelly, and Galanter (1981) estimated the exponent using a signal detection procedure. By varying the (small) payoffs for hits, correct rejections, false alarms, and misses Kornbrot et al (1981) estimate an exponent of 0.48.…”
Section: How the Distribution Of Attributementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kornbrot, Donnelly, and Galanter (1981) estimated the exponent using a signal detection procedure. By varying the (small) payoffs for hits, correct rejections, false alarms, and misses Kornbrot et al (1981) estimate an exponent of 0.48. Galanter (1990) repeated his earlier procedure and found an exponent of 0.54.…”
Section: How the Distribution Of Attributementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Measure 1* comes from normative statistical decision theory, and was argued for by Ingham (1970) and Treisman (1964). Measures 2* and 3* are of importance in connection with additive learning models: Some models of this family predict probability matching, P(r1) = y, which is a special case of (3*), and others predict (2*) (see Dusoir, 1980, andKombrot, Donnelly, &Galanter, 1981). Craig's (1976) model, and a special case of Ambler's (1976) model, also assume [p(r1»)' (4*) comes from Healy and Jones (1973).…”
Section: Tony Dusoir Ulster Polytechnic Newtownabbey Northern Irelandmentioning
confidence: 99%