2000
DOI: 10.1006/cogp.2000.0738
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Expectancy, Attention, and Time

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

31
340
6
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 289 publications
(378 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
31
340
6
1
Order By: Relevance
“…vanMarle and Wynn (2006) reported that 6-month-old infants can discriminate event durations between 2 and 4 s. Brannon et al (2004Brannon et al ( , 2008 additionally showed that 10-month-old infants can detect changes in temporal rhythm by detecting a temporal deviation in a stream of tones formed by a regular inter-stimulus interval. In terms of infants' ability to benefit from rhythmic and regular patterns, their behavior is similar to that observed in adult research (Large and Jones, 1999;Barnes and Jones, 2000;Sanabria et al, 2011).…”
Section: Stages Of Development Of the Attention Systemsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…vanMarle and Wynn (2006) reported that 6-month-old infants can discriminate event durations between 2 and 4 s. Brannon et al (2004Brannon et al ( , 2008 additionally showed that 10-month-old infants can detect changes in temporal rhythm by detecting a temporal deviation in a stream of tones formed by a regular inter-stimulus interval. In terms of infants' ability to benefit from rhythmic and regular patterns, their behavior is similar to that observed in adult research (Large and Jones, 1999;Barnes and Jones, 2000;Sanabria et al, 2011).…”
Section: Stages Of Development Of the Attention Systemsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Large and Jones (1999) found that time change detection accuracy was better when stimuli occurred at unexpected times and that duration estimation accuracy was better when stimulus events occurred at expected times, where expected times were defined by DAT. Barnes and Jones (2000) found that duration judgments about a standard and a comparison interval were most accurate when the temporal context preceding the standard had a simple harmonic relationship to the standard interval. In all of these studies, temporal context altered sensitivity to temporal information in a direction consistent with a coupled oscillator mechanism.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Such a concept implies a dynamic change, and indeed, many dynamic phenomena related to attention have been described. We investigated the ability of deterministic oscillator and stochastic timing models to account for the temporal allocation of attention, using both qualitative and quantitative approaches to chronometric data.A theoretical account of dynamic attention by Jones (1976) has been the focus of several recent studies (Barnes & Jones, 2000;Large & Jones, 1999;McAuley & Kidd, 1998;Olsen & Chun, 2001). According to dynamic attending theory (DAT;Jones, 1976;Jones & Boltz, 1989;Large & Jones, 1999), attention is guided through time by internal oscillators termed attending rhythms.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when throwing a ball at a target, the spatial accuracy relies, at least partially, on precise motor timing; however, subjects may not be explicitly aware of the timing of individual components of this complex multijoint movement (1,2). Similarly, subjects may improve the speed and accuracy of performing a perceptual task by implicitly using temporal information to predict the timing of sensory stimuli (i.e., temporal expectancy) (3,4). More directly, implicit timing can be conceptualized as a critical component of classical conditioning and other stimulus/response association processes such as implicit learning and automatic behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%