1997
DOI: 10.1016/s0140-6736(05)61595-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

False-positive self-reports of HIV infection

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In developed countries, studies among populations at high risk (such as drug users, prostitutes and prisoners) suggests that concurrence between individuals' self-reports of current HIV status and their HIV test results is high for sero-negative people (95-99%) but low for sero-positive people (40-70%) [9-12]. A study of attendees at a voluntary HIV testing centre in Zambia found a 30% rate of incorrect self-reports, with sero-positive patients being only slightly more accurate than sero-negative patients (72% v 60%) [13]. In contrast, a case-control study in Tanzania found no significant difference between perceived risk of infection and HIV status [14].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In developed countries, studies among populations at high risk (such as drug users, prostitutes and prisoners) suggests that concurrence between individuals' self-reports of current HIV status and their HIV test results is high for sero-negative people (95-99%) but low for sero-positive people (40-70%) [9-12]. A study of attendees at a voluntary HIV testing centre in Zambia found a 30% rate of incorrect self-reports, with sero-positive patients being only slightly more accurate than sero-negative patients (72% v 60%) [13]. In contrast, a case-control study in Tanzania found no significant difference between perceived risk of infection and HIV status [14].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Inaccuracies seem to be largely driven by ‘false positives’, that is, individuals who perceive themselves to be at an elevated risk of HIV/AIDS, but who are, in fact, uninfected. In another example, 33% of clients at a VCT center in Zambia gave incorrect self reports of their HIV/AIDS status, and 53% of these inaccurate self-reports were for individuals who thought they were HIV positive but were in fact HIV negative (Chintu et al 1997). Because those seeking VCT may do so because they are already experiencing symptoms of AIDS, it is not surprising that false-positive HIV assessments were slightly more common at a VCT center than in a population-based sample: in rural Malawi, approximately 26% of respondents incorrectly thought they were HIV positive, and 92.8% of those who thought they were infected were actually HIV negative (Bignami-Van Assche et al 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A study of attenders at a voluntary HIV testing centre in Zambia found a 30% rate of incorrect self-reports, with seropositive patients being only slightly more accurate than seronegative patients (72% v 60%). 7 In contrast, a case-control study in Tanzania found no significant association between perceived risk of infection and HIV status. 8 In this paper, we examine the validity of self-reported likelihood of current HIV infection among the general population in rural Malawi by comparing survey responses with the results of oral fluid assays for HIV antibodies.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%