2004
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-004-2128-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feeling what you hear: auditory signals can modulate tactile tap perception

Abstract: We tested whether auditory sequences of beeps can modulate the tactile perception of sequences of taps (two to four taps per sequence) delivered to the index fingertip. In the first experiment, the auditory and tactile sequences were presented simultaneously. The number of beeps delivered in the auditory sequence were either the same as, less than, or more than the number of taps of the simultaneously presented tactile sequence. Though task-irrelevant (subjects were instructed to focus on the tactile stimuli),… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

14
87
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 151 publications
(102 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
14
87
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Andersen, Tiippana, and Sams (2004) extended this work by showing that the number of perceived flashes can be either increased (called fission) or decreased (called fusion) by presenting either a larger or a smaller number of irrelevant beeps in combination with the flashes. In addition to these audiovisual illusions, comparable effects have been reported for almost all other combinations of modalities (Bresciani et al, 2005;Courtney, Motes, & Hubbard, 2007;Ernst, Bresciani, Drewing, & Bülthoff, 2004;Hötting & Röder, 2004;Violentyev, Shimojo, & Shams, 2005).…”
supporting
confidence: 63%
“…Andersen, Tiippana, and Sams (2004) extended this work by showing that the number of perceived flashes can be either increased (called fission) or decreased (called fusion) by presenting either a larger or a smaller number of irrelevant beeps in combination with the flashes. In addition to these audiovisual illusions, comparable effects have been reported for almost all other combinations of modalities (Bresciani et al, 2005;Courtney, Motes, & Hubbard, 2007;Ernst, Bresciani, Drewing, & Bülthoff, 2004;Hötting & Röder, 2004;Violentyev, Shimojo, & Shams, 2005).…”
supporting
confidence: 63%
“…A similar phenomenon is evident in the tendencies to both perceive the rate of a rapidly fluttering bimodal rhythmic stimulus to be that of the auditory, rather than the visual, component (Recanzone, 2003;Welch, DuttonHurt & Warren, 1986) and to misperceive the number of taps to the skin in line with the number of accompanying auditory beeps (Bresciani et al, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…The temporal compression observed under intentional (or causal) binding could then be explained as an instance of the more general phenomenon of multisensory integration, which involves the merging of cues from different modalities into a single percept (Ernst & Bülthoff, 2004). This occurs only within a small temporal window around simultaneity, often called the temporal window of integration (Shams et al, 2002;Bresciani et al, 2005). The 'S'-shaped compressive bias around simultaneity could well be a consequence of this window of temporal integration.…”
Section: Visuomotor Temporal Recalibration and Intentional Bindingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intentional binding likely contributes to the unity assumption (Welch & Warren, 1980), which is a prerequisite for multisensory integration. Integration typically requires stimuli to occur in close temporal proximity, i.e., they should fall within a window of integration (e.g., Shams et al, 2002;Bresciani et al 2005). If intentional or causal binding only occurs for discrepancies where movement leads the temporal order, this could lead to asymmetries in processing or recalibration of visuomotor time perception, due to an asymmetrical window of integration.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%