2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Gender differences on general knowledge tests: Are they due to Differential Item Functioning?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In order to compare our sample with the norm samples of the instruments, we also calculated the T values ( M = 50, SD = 10) and the standard deviations of the T values (for the personality factors, the norm samples for 16- to 20-year old men and women, respectively, from 2007 [56] were used, because there is no standardization for the German NEO-FFI edition from 1993; the instrument itself remained unchanged from 1993 to 2007). From the T values, it can be seen that the sample was slightly above average, and showed some variance restriction, in intelligence, which is typical for students from Gymnasiums [57,58,59]. Regarding personality, all the factors but O matched the mean of the norming samples quite well and did not show any greater restriction in variance.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 89%
“…In order to compare our sample with the norm samples of the instruments, we also calculated the T values ( M = 50, SD = 10) and the standard deviations of the T values (for the personality factors, the norm samples for 16- to 20-year old men and women, respectively, from 2007 [56] were used, because there is no standardization for the German NEO-FFI edition from 1993; the instrument itself remained unchanged from 1993 to 2007). From the T values, it can be seen that the sample was slightly above average, and showed some variance restriction, in intelligence, which is typical for students from Gymnasiums [57,58,59]. Regarding personality, all the factors but O matched the mean of the norming samples quite well and did not show any greater restriction in variance.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 89%
“…However, as with measurement invariance and differential item functioning research, sometimes it is unclear why a violation occurs. This can then be addressed in follow-up research (e.g., explaining the invariance using covariates, see Steinmayr, Bergold, Margraf-Stiksrud, & Freund, 2015).…”
Section: Exploratory and Confirmatory Usementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, while CTT showed a gender gap in 81% of sample countries and territories, IRT shows a gender gap in only 54% (21 out of 39) of the sample. These results are consistent with other knowledge testing assessments which also suffer from DIF for example the gender gap in political knowledge (Lizotte and Sidman, 2009) and high school educational testing (Steinmayr et al ., 2015).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a gender‐fair test, the relation between the ability being measured and test behavior should not be influenced by gender (Steinmayr et al ., 2015). Yet it is widely observed that women are more likely than men to choose the “do not know” response in a multiple choice test (Agnew et al ., 2008; Lusardi et al ., 2010; Bucher‐Koenen et al ., 2017; Chen and Garand, 2018).…”
Section: Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation