2006
DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arj062
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Habitat assessment by parasitoids: mechanisms for patch use behavior

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
30
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
3
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with assumptions from rate-maximizing models, mated L. testaceipes females are time limited (Tentelier et al 2006;van Steenis 1994), and in agreement with predictions from the MVT, their residence time and the number of aphids they attack on a colony increase with increasing number of aphids . Here, we will show that this is not the case for virgin females foraging in the wild.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 74%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Consistent with assumptions from rate-maximizing models, mated L. testaceipes females are time limited (Tentelier et al 2006;van Steenis 1994), and in agreement with predictions from the MVT, their residence time and the number of aphids they attack on a colony increase with increasing number of aphids . Here, we will show that this is not the case for virgin females foraging in the wild.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…An attack occurs when the female bends her abdomen between her legs and stings an aphid. In laboratory experiments on patch exploitation, 78% of attacks result in the development of a parasitoid (Tentelier et al 2006). Each observation was stopped when one of the following conditions was met: (1) the female had been found and mated by a wild male; (2) 1,800 s had elapsed since the release; or (3) the female had left the colony (i.e., it had departed the focal leaf for more than 60 s), whichever happened Wrst.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies show seasonal variation in foraging patterns (e.g., Conner, 1981;Owen-Smith, 1994;van Marken Lichtenbelt, 1993), but do nonhuman animals use recent experience to guide foraging behavior? Studies have found that poor foraging experiences (e.g., low target quantity at a particular location) can lead to longer search durations in the subsequent foraging episode (Tentelier, Desouhant, & Fauvergue, 2006;Visser, van Alphen, & Nell, 1992), consistent with the predictions of a moment-tomoment optimal foraging model. But what would happen if predictable, short-term temporal signals were given to nonhuman animals?…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…More generally, information helps the forager to estimate both local parameters, patch quality (Iwasa et al 1981;Green 1980;Olsson and Brown 2006) and large-scale modiWcations of the habitat conditions (Mangel 1990). For example, only two key pieces of information are needed to estimate the average rate of gain in the habitat: interpatch travel time and the quality of the previous patches (Tentelier et al 2006). This information is gleaned during the forager's experience.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, remembering what was encountered in previous patches could help them to more accurately estimate both the subsequent patch quality and the average patch quality in the environment. Many parasitoid species have been shown to modify their decision to leave a patch according to their oV-patch experience, such as interpatch travel time and previous patch quality (Visser et al 1992;Wajnberg et al 2006;Outreman et al 2005;van Baaren et al 2005a, b;Tentelier et al 2006;Thiel and HoVmeister 2006;Muratori et al 2008;Thiel 2011). Taking this into consideration, it is expected that parasitoids could use memory to retain quantitative information about the previous visited patch in order to assess the subsequent patch quality relatively.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%