This study examined the effects of category structure and category size on categorization reaction time, artificial categories being used to obtain greater experimental control than is usually found in semantic memory research. Four artificial categories varying in structure (hierarchical and nonhierarchical) and size (8 or 16 instances) were introduced to 20 adolescent subjects over a 12-week period by means of stories, exercises, and discussions. Significantly longer categorization reaction times were required for instances from the hierarchical categories, but no set size effect was found. The application of clustering and multidimensional scaling procedures to subjects' free recall data revealed that subjects had acquired the hierarchical structures but imposed their own structures on the other categories. These findings were interpreted within a spreading activation framework.Research in semantic memory has relied heavily on the natural memories of subjects, and relatively few studies have attempted to use artificial information. The few include Smith, Haviland, Buckley, and Sack (1972), who used learned artificial facts in order to examine the confounding in the Collins and Quillian (1969) memory organization research between noun-property frequency and number of hypothesized deductive steps, and Potts (1976), who examined the ability of subjects to correctly accept or reject assertions based on previously learned artificial logical relationships. Recently, Murphy and Smith (1982) used artificial category names in order to control for both the length and familiarity of natural category names. None of these experiments, however, has examined the spontaneous generation of a semantic memory structure for unfamiliar stimulus material over a lengthy training period.Awareness of problems associated with the use of natural memories has led to both caution and debate over the control of relevant variables. Landauer and Meyer (1972) discussed the problems encountered in determining word frequency and relative sizesof semantic categories, as well as the confounding introduced by This paper is based on an MA thesis submitted to the University of Newcastle by the first author and supervised by the second author. We wish to thank the subjects for their generous participation in the project for a period of almost 12 months. words with distinctive phonetic markers (e.g., "ology") and categories with an unusually large proportion of short or long words. Anderson and Bower (1973) doubted if factors such as word frequency, conjoint propositional freq uency, recency, and concreteness could be adequately controlled.The debate over measurement of category size illustrates the seemingly intractable control problem using natural memories. Methods of measuring category size can be summarized as being either analytic or subject-based (Wilkins, 1979). The analytic methods are illustrated by the use of nested pairs of categories, as in Landauer and Freedman (1968), and the counting of exemplars in some compendium of English words, s...