2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.10.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: The extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry

Abstract: Performance in visual tasks is limited by the low-level mechanisms that sample the visual field. It is well documented that contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution decrease as a function of eccentricity and that those factors impair performance in “higher level” tasks, such as visual search. Performance also varies consistently at isoeccentric locations in the visual field. Specifically, at a fixed eccentricity, performance is better along the horizontal meridian than the vertical meridian, and along the l… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

41
188
6
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 152 publications
(236 citation statements)
references
References 86 publications
41
188
6
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This effect was observed in both hemifields. This type of visual field asymmetry has been reported previously (19)(20)(21)(22). As a result, two probe locations, even adjacent, might present different report probabilities (i.e., P1 > P2) due to a position bias, rather than the kind of attentional effect under investigation here (one might speculate as to whether the position bias is, in turn, due to an attentional effect or some other spatially variable aspect of visual processing, but this question is beyond the scope of this investigation).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This effect was observed in both hemifields. This type of visual field asymmetry has been reported previously (19)(20)(21)(22). As a result, two probe locations, even adjacent, might present different report probabilities (i.e., P1 > P2) due to a position bias, rather than the kind of attentional effect under investigation here (one might speculate as to whether the position bias is, in turn, due to an attentional effect or some other spatially variable aspect of visual processing, but this question is beyond the scope of this investigation).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…A study in the macaque monkey showed that neurons in the frontal eye field respond sequentially to the items of a difficult search task, at a frequency of [18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25]. This finding was the first strong evidence of a link between attentional periodicity and sequential processing in visual search.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Contrary to recent suggestions that crowding and saccades are closely linked (37)(38)(39), four aspects of our data lead us to reject an inextricable linkage between these processes. First, although crowding and saccades show similar patterns of variation, saccadic precision is worse in the lower visual field than in the upper-the opposite pattern not only to crowding, but also to a wide range of perceptual tasks (24,25). Second, our participants were clearly able to trade their precision between the two processes; saccadic precision was highest in trials where crowded identification was incorrect and vice versa.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Eye movements are similarly affected -both the distribution of landing errors for saccadic eye movements (20) and the detection of position shifts in saccadic targets (21) show these characteristics. Additionally, as with crowding, performance is worse in the upper than in the lower visual field for gap resolution and Vernier acuity (22,23), along with a range of other spatial identification measures (24,25) that similarly show worse performance along the vertical than the horizontal meridian.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…The left/right advantage is thought to reflect the asymmetrical organization of the human visual system (Abrams, Nizam, & Carrasco, 2012;Carrasco, 2014;Kobayashi & Kohshima, 1997, 2001aNomura, Katahata, & Hashiya, 2005;Ossandón, Onat, & König, 2014;Pauszek & Gibson, 2016; see also Robertson, 2004, for a review). In the context of the spatial cuing paradigm, the left/right advantage manifests as faster RTs due to more efficient target processing when the target appears at the left or right location, relative to when the target appears at the above or below location.…”
Section: The Left/right Advantagementioning
confidence: 99%