2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.08.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learned color taste associations in a repeated brief exposure paradigm

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
27
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
2
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Perhaps the most parsimonious explanation for the ubiquity of colour-taste correspondences is in terms of the internalization of the crossmodal statistics of the environment (e.g., Barlow, 2001;Ernst, 2007). There is, for example, evidence from both adults (Higgins & Hayes, 2019) and also 4-month-old babies (Reardon & Bushnell, 1988) that, regardless of their age, people rapidly pick up novel associations between colours and basic tastes. It would certainly seem unlikely that we have developed hardwired associations between specific colours and particular tastes, given the changing association between taste/flavour and colour both across cultures, and over the course of history (e.g., Hisano, 2019;Woolgar, 2018; see also Wilson, 1991).…”
Section: Internalizing the Crossmodal Statistics Of The Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Perhaps the most parsimonious explanation for the ubiquity of colour-taste correspondences is in terms of the internalization of the crossmodal statistics of the environment (e.g., Barlow, 2001;Ernst, 2007). There is, for example, evidence from both adults (Higgins & Hayes, 2019) and also 4-month-old babies (Reardon & Bushnell, 1988) that, regardless of their age, people rapidly pick up novel associations between colours and basic tastes. It would certainly seem unlikely that we have developed hardwired associations between specific colours and particular tastes, given the changing association between taste/flavour and colour both across cultures, and over the course of history (e.g., Hisano, 2019;Woolgar, 2018; see also Wilson, 1991).…”
Section: Internalizing the Crossmodal Statistics Of The Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, one might consider the crossmodal associations between colour and basic taste, or flavours, in much the same light [100,135]. That is, people may well associate a pinkish-red colour with sweetness not because they perceive the component stimuli to be similar, but rather because, in the absence of any other information, they expect pinkish-red foods to taste sweet, as a result of a learned association [101,[135][136][137]. It is an open question as to whether such crossmodal predictive coding [138,139] should necessarily be labelled as a kind of crossmodal correspondence or not.…”
Section: Pairing Of Music and Food/beverage Stimuli Based On Perceivementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other psychophysical data in humans also suggest that bitter stimuli can be differentiated based on their temporal [3,8,9] and regional perception [4,10]. However, in a learning task designed to condition participants to associate a taste (i.e., one sweet, three bitter) with a specific color cue, participants were only able to associate the sweet sample significantly above chance; that is, there was no evidence for discrimination between the bitterants in a brief conditioning paradigm [11]. Collectively, these mixed findings suggest a need for additional research to investigate the possibility of multiple bitter percepts in humans.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%