2009
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-1764-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning the visuomotor transformation of virtual and real sliding levers: simple approximations of complex transformations

Abstract: Learning to operate a complex tool such as a sliding lever can be conceived as learning both a kinematic and a dynamic transformation. We investigated whether the presence of the dynamic transformation has an inhibitory or a facilitative effect on learning to control a sliding lever. Furthermore, we examined the characteristics of the internal model of the kinematic transformation of the lever in visual open-loop trials. In the experiment, one group of participants practiced with only the kinematic transformat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
29
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
2
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This means that there is a novel relation of ones' own movements to their visually perceived consequences such as the location of the impact of the head of the hammer or the position of the cursor on the computer monitor. During recent years evidence has been accumulated which points to the existence of rapid discrete and slower continuous processes of adaptation (e.g., Abeele & Bock, 2001;Bock, Abeele, & Eversheim, 2003;Sülzenbrück & Heuer, 2009aWerner & Bock, 2010), which are perhaps related to discrete and continuous processes of motor planning (Ghez et al, 1997). Typically discrete processes approximate the visuo-motor transformation, and continuous processes incrementally increase the accuracy of the initial approximation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…This means that there is a novel relation of ones' own movements to their visually perceived consequences such as the location of the impact of the head of the hammer or the position of the cursor on the computer monitor. During recent years evidence has been accumulated which points to the existence of rapid discrete and slower continuous processes of adaptation (e.g., Abeele & Bock, 2001;Bock, Abeele, & Eversheim, 2003;Sülzenbrück & Heuer, 2009aWerner & Bock, 2010), which are perhaps related to discrete and continuous processes of motor planning (Ghez et al, 1997). Typically discrete processes approximate the visuo-motor transformation, and continuous processes incrementally increase the accuracy of the initial approximation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The adjustments of the final hand positions were inaccurate, but they were clearly present. The inaccuracy is not that remarkable because the internal model of the kinematic transformation of a sliding lever is of limited accuracy in general (e.g., Sülzenbrück and Heuer 2009, 2012. Even for the simple transverse movements, the required learning of position-dependent visuomotor gains is likely a challenging task (cf.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Referring to the previously mentioned limited accuracy of the internal model of the complex lever transformation (e.g., Sülzenbrück and Heuer 2009, 2012, it could be argued that especially such a complex tool as the sliding lever needs more time to be incorporated into the body schema and that with a less complex tool different results would have been obtained. Of course, longer training periods could perhaps change our pattern of results.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to intentions and to processes that are inaccessible to conscious awareness, there are processes and their outcomes that are accessible, though typically they are automatic. For example, Voigt (1933) described a fairly undifferentiated (Sülzenbrück, & Heuer, 2009a) awareness of jumps, which he-as a doctoral student of Otto Klemm and the Leipzig school of ''Ganzheitspsychologie''-characterizes as a motor Gestalt. But the motor Gestalt is fragile and can break up, e.g.…”
Section: Conscious Awareness Beyond Intentions and Movement Effectsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In addition, the inaccuracy of internal models can be systematic in that the model is a specific simplified approximation of the transformation. Figure 3 shows an example (Sülzenbrück, & Heuer, 2009a). Participants in that study had to learn to control the distal tip of a sliding lever by moving its proximal end with the hand, with the position of the distal tip of the lever being presented by a cursor on a monitor.…”
Section: Post-intentional Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%