1989
DOI: 10.1080/0022027890210501
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning to read in Japan

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0
1

Year Published

1995
1995
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
3
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Methods by which the characters are taught include writing in the air, color charts showing radicals (the common elements under which characters are grouped), rote practice on squared sheets of paper, build-up methods and a range of other techniques (Bourke, 1996: 167-174). the whole-class method observed by Mason et al (1989) remains normal practice, with an emphasis on both oral and silent reading and on teacher-centred practices. Much as children whose first language is english achieve variable results in spelling, the ideal of perfect retention of characters and their readings may not always be reached: a 1988 survey report on children in three of Japan's 47 prefectures, for example, showed that at both elementary and high school levels students were better able to recognize than to reproduce kanji by hand, and that knowledge of on and kun readings fluctuated (Kokuritsu Kokugo Kenkyūjo, 1988: 389-391).…”
Section: Japanesementioning
confidence: 98%
“…Methods by which the characters are taught include writing in the air, color charts showing radicals (the common elements under which characters are grouped), rote practice on squared sheets of paper, build-up methods and a range of other techniques (Bourke, 1996: 167-174). the whole-class method observed by Mason et al (1989) remains normal practice, with an emphasis on both oral and silent reading and on teacher-centred practices. Much as children whose first language is english achieve variable results in spelling, the ideal of perfect retention of characters and their readings may not always be reached: a 1988 survey report on children in three of Japan's 47 prefectures, for example, showed that at both elementary and high school levels students were better able to recognize than to reproduce kanji by hand, and that knowledge of on and kun readings fluctuated (Kokuritsu Kokugo Kenkyūjo, 1988: 389-391).…”
Section: Japanesementioning
confidence: 98%
“…(Historically, conscious efforts were often made by spelling reformers to avoid homophones becoming homographs and thereby remove possible ambiguity (Carney, 1994, chapter 7;Scragg, 1974, chapter 4). 6 Purely phonemic scripts (such as pinyin and Zhu-Yin-Fu-Hao) as well as syllabic/moraic scripts (such as Japanese kana) appear to be remarkably easy to learn to decode (e.g., Mason, Anderson, Omura, Uchida, & Imai, 1989;McCarthy, 1995;Taylor & Taylor, 1995), but if extensive homophony exists in the spoken language, such scripts will violate the morphemic distinctiveness principle and prove impracticable without supplementary morpheme-based characters (as in the case of Japanese Kanji and Korean Hancha) that provide more direct links to morpheme identity. By the same token, i.t.a.…”
Section: Where Are the Jabberwockies?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is reasonable to suppose that in the United States, given the nature of the English language and our letter-and word-focused beginning reading instruction (Mason, Anderson, Omura, Uchida, & Imai, 1990), reading to children might be a way to introduce literacy (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott, & Wilkinson, 1985). Hearing and discussing texts with literate persons could help young children establish connections between oral language and written text structures and maintain a sense of text meaningfulness (Holdaway, 1979(Holdaway, , 1986Sulzby, 1986aSulzby, , 1986b.…”
Section: Reading Stories To Preliterate Children: a Proposed Connectimentioning
confidence: 99%