2018
DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lose-Shift Responding in Humans Is Promoted by Increased Cognitive Load

Abstract: The propensity of animals to shift choices immediately after unexpectedly poor reinforcement outcomes is a pervasive strategy across species and tasks. We report here on the memory supporting such lose-shift responding in humans, assessed using a binary choice task in which random responding is the optimal strategy. Participants exhibited little lose-shift responding when fully attending to the task, but this increased by 30%–40% in participants that performed with additional cognitive load that is known to ta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
17
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
2
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Potentially, the disadvantageous choices could be caused solely by negative outcomes of a preceding advantageous choice, which could prompt the participants to immediately change their preference (Gaffan & Davies, 1981;Ivan, Banks, Goodfellow, & Gruber, 2018) i.e., to follow a Win-Stay Lose-Shift strategy (see e.g., Ellerby & Tunney, 2017). In order to check whether the disadvantageous choices were mainly caused by a previous loss, we investigated probabilities of transitions leading from the advantageous to the disadvantageous choice.…”
Section: Trial Selection Criteria and The Factors Of Interestmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Potentially, the disadvantageous choices could be caused solely by negative outcomes of a preceding advantageous choice, which could prompt the participants to immediately change their preference (Gaffan & Davies, 1981;Ivan, Banks, Goodfellow, & Gruber, 2018) i.e., to follow a Win-Stay Lose-Shift strategy (see e.g., Ellerby & Tunney, 2017). In order to check whether the disadvantageous choices were mainly caused by a previous loss, we investigated probabilities of transitions leading from the advantageous to the disadvantageous choice.…”
Section: Trial Selection Criteria and The Factors Of Interestmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As such, the internal phasic state generated by positive outcomes along with the external experience of switching opponents makes it more likely that better performance will be expressed. In addition to extending time between trials [58], switching opponents can be added to the toolkit that helps to reduce the expression of typical reinforcement learning rules, thereby helping individuals who may be at their most cognitively vulnerable within competitive environments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While the data from Experiment 2 suggest a somewhat pessimistic (inhibitory) role of interruption during competition, it is clear that similar kinds of decision-making might be naturally interrupted after the experience of a positive outcome either by the player in the form of post-reinforcement pausing (e.g., Dixon & Schreiber, 2004;Dyson et al, 2018;Verbruggen et al, 2017;Forder & Dyson, 2016) or by the opponent itself such as in the case of slot machines where longer music tends to play when the win is bigger (e.g., Dixon et al, 2013). This leads us to the intriguing possibility that mandatory pauses following negative outcomes during play conditions might help to break the cyclical poorer-quality decision making characterised in problem gambling (see Ivan, Banks, Goodfellow & Gruber, 2018, for a similar suggestion).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%