2012
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611430953
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring the Prevalence of Questionable Research Practices With Incentives for Truth Telling

Abstract: Cases of clear scientific misconduct have received significant media attention recently, but less flagrantly questionable research practices may be more prevalent and, ultimately, more damaging to the academic enterprise. Using an anonymous elicitation format supplemented by incentives for honest reporting, we surveyed over 2,000 psychologists about their involvement in questionable research practices. The impact of truth-telling incentives on self-admissions of questionable research practices was positive, an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

34
1,870
9
49

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,975 publications
(1,962 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
34
1,870
9
49
Order By: Relevance
“…This intuition is correct from an accuracy point of view: More data leads to more precise estimates (e.g., Maxwell, Kelley, & Rausch, 2008;Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013). According to John, Loewenstein, and Prelec (2012), optionally increasing the sample size when the results are not significant is one of the most common (questionable) research practices. Furthermore, Yu, Sprenger, Thomas, and Dougherty (2013) showed empirically which 4 SCHÖNBRODT (incorrect) heuristics researchers used in their optional stopping practice.…”
Section: The Nhst Procedures With a Priori Power Analysis And Some Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This intuition is correct from an accuracy point of view: More data leads to more precise estimates (e.g., Maxwell, Kelley, & Rausch, 2008;Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013). According to John, Loewenstein, and Prelec (2012), optionally increasing the sample size when the results are not significant is one of the most common (questionable) research practices. Furthermore, Yu, Sprenger, Thomas, and Dougherty (2013) showed empirically which 4 SCHÖNBRODT (incorrect) heuristics researchers used in their optional stopping practice.…”
Section: The Nhst Procedures With a Priori Power Analysis And Some Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The correct way to incorporate the results of data collected after the analysis of an initial tranche of data is via meta-analysis (Braver et al 2014). Just adding the new data to the existing data set is wrong, since it involves deciding to collect more data after looking at the results (John et al 2012). Equally, meta-analysis of all six studies is not a valid approach because the five studies in the first tranche were planned in advance (before the experiment) as defined in the protocol.…”
Section: Phase 2 Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, questionable research practices such as manipulating statistical analyses until desired results are produced (i.e., data fishing and p-hacking; see https://projects.fivethirtyeight.com/p-hacking/ for an interactive demonstration), hypothesizing after results are known (i.e., HARKing), and selectively reporting analyses and publishing studies with positive results (i.e., reporting and publication bias) are not so clearly proscribed. Researchers report engaging in these questionable research practices, PROMOTING OPEN SCIENCE 8 which can affect study findings and influence the trustworthiness of evidence (John et al, 2012;Simmons et al, 2011).…”
Section: Concerns About the Culture Of Contemporary Sciencementioning
confidence: 99%