1984
DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90172-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: Two different modes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
123
5
1

Year Published

1989
1989
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 264 publications
(141 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
12
123
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, averaging saccades, in general, have shorter latencies than target-directed saccades (Chou et al 1999;Coeffe and O'Regan 1987;Findlay 1981aFindlay , 1997Jacobs 1987;Ottes et al 1984Ottes et al , 1985Walker et al 1997), whereas in our study, the latencies of the corrective saccades had an almost linear relation with their final destinationsincreasing from the saccades that went to the old position to those that went mid-way and finally to those that went to the new position of the final target. In other words, for all subjects, the latencies of midway corrective saccades (mean across subjects ϭ 308 ms) were longer than the latencies of those directed to the old position (mean across subjects ϭ 298 ms) but shorter than the latencies of saccades directed to the new position of the final target (mean across subjects ϭ 354 ms).…”
Section: Decisions and Motor Planning During Error Correctioncontrasting
confidence: 38%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, averaging saccades, in general, have shorter latencies than target-directed saccades (Chou et al 1999;Coeffe and O'Regan 1987;Findlay 1981aFindlay , 1997Jacobs 1987;Ottes et al 1984Ottes et al , 1985Walker et al 1997), whereas in our study, the latencies of the corrective saccades had an almost linear relation with their final destinationsincreasing from the saccades that went to the old position to those that went mid-way and finally to those that went to the new position of the final target. In other words, for all subjects, the latencies of midway corrective saccades (mean across subjects ϭ 308 ms) were longer than the latencies of those directed to the old position (mean across subjects ϭ 298 ms) but shorter than the latencies of saccades directed to the new position of the final target (mean across subjects ϭ 354 ms).…”
Section: Decisions and Motor Planning During Error Correctioncontrasting
confidence: 38%
“…Instances of saccades being directed at a location midway between two simultaneously or sequentially presented targets have been previously reported (Becker and Jurgens 1979;Chou et al 1999;Ottes et al 1984) and is particularly observed when the angular distance between two targets is Ͻ30° (Ottes et al 1985) as in our study. However, averaging saccades, in general, have shorter latencies than target-directed saccades (Chou et al 1999;Coeffe and O'Regan 1987;Findlay 1981aFindlay , 1997Jacobs 1987;Ottes et al 1984Ottes et al , 1985Walker et al 1997), whereas in our study, the latencies of the corrective saccades had an almost linear relation with their final destinationsincreasing from the saccades that went to the old position to those that went mid-way and finally to those that went to the new position of the final target.…”
Section: Decisions and Motor Planning During Error Correctionmentioning
confidence: 50%
“…It is clear that workers studying reading have in the past made use of principles derived from basic research on saccade generation. Examples of this are the analogy between the double-step studies and the SAS theories; the use of the concept of the saccadic range effect (Kapoula & Robinson, 1986) in McConkie et al's (1988) explanation of saccadic landing positions in reading; and O'Regan's (1990) use of the "global" or "centre of gravity" effect (Findlay, 1982;Ottes et al 1984;Deubel et al, 1984) in his strategy-tactics theory. However, many of the principles that have been appealed to were based on experimental work using very simple stimuli and tasks quite unlike those found in normal reading.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Depending on the timing, size and direction of this second target step, different effects on the saccadic behaviour can be observed -both the spatial end position and the latency of the saccade may be affected by the interfering target displacement, in characteristically different ways. Depending on its relative size and direction, the perturbing stimulus induces basically different effects, which led Ottes, van Gisbergen, & Eggermont (1984) to postulate two different "modes", one, in which averaging responses occur, and the other, in which a bistable response pattern prevails. The findings may be summarised as follows:…”
Section: What Can Be Learned From Basic Research On Saccade Programming?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2.4, the model does not cover the process of saccade generation itself. Previous models of saccade generation using DNFs (Kopecz and Schöner 1995;Wilimzig et al 2006) have addressed how the metric distance between saccade targets determines whether selection or averaging occurs, including in double-step paradigms (Ottes et al 1984;Aslin and Shea 1987). DNF models have addressed repulsion and attraction effects between metrically close items in related work on spatial working memory (Simmering et al 2008).…”
Section: Trans-saccadic Spatial Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%