2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.02.018
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pedestrian-driver communication and decision strategies at marked crossings

Abstract: The aim of this work is to describe pedestrian-driver encounters, communication, and decision strategies at marked but unsignalised crossings in urban areas in the Czech Republic and the ways in which the parties involved experience and handle these encounters. A mixed-methods design was used, consisting of focus groups with pedestrians and drivers regarding their subjective views of the situations, on-site observations, camera recordings, speed measurements, the measurement of car and pedestrian densities, an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

9
153
3
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 234 publications
(167 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
9
153
3
2
Order By: Relevance
“…One important part of the process is thought to be a series of cognitive processes that allow people to estimate the internal mental states and to predict future actions of others (Sebanz et al, 2006). In close proximity encounters, such as zebra crossings and parking lots, the cognitive processes involved are highly based on non-verbal communication with drivers; by interpreting cues in a driver’s behavior such as eye contact and gestures, pedestrians can reliably predict their intentions and the near-term movements of the vehicle (Schmidt and Färber, 2009; Sucha et al, 2017). By contrast, when it comes to assessing the imminent actions of AVs, pedestrians do not currently possess comparable intuitive abilities.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…One important part of the process is thought to be a series of cognitive processes that allow people to estimate the internal mental states and to predict future actions of others (Sebanz et al, 2006). In close proximity encounters, such as zebra crossings and parking lots, the cognitive processes involved are highly based on non-verbal communication with drivers; by interpreting cues in a driver’s behavior such as eye contact and gestures, pedestrians can reliably predict their intentions and the near-term movements of the vehicle (Schmidt and Färber, 2009; Sucha et al, 2017). By contrast, when it comes to assessing the imminent actions of AVs, pedestrians do not currently possess comparable intuitive abilities.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is known that these interactions are complex and often affected by various factors ( Figure 1 ) such as: vehicle speed and time to collision (Várhelyi, 1998; Sun et al, 2015; Schneemann and Gohl, 2016), traffic density and size of the gap between the vehicles (Wang et al, 2010), road features such as geometry and signs (Knoblauch et al, 1996), weather and light conditions (Sun et al, 2015), crossing speed (Knoblauch et al, 1996), presence and behavior of other road users (Rosenbloom, 2009; Zhou et al, 2009), demographics of drivers and pedestrians (Oxley et al, 2005; Lobjois and Cavallo, 2007; Tom and Granié, 2011), as well as their experiences, knowledge, motivations, and cognitive state (Mwakalonge et al, 2015). In addition, expectations and feelings of safety or insecurity affect the way the interactions develop (Zhou et al, 2009; Malmsten Lundgren et al, 2017; Sucha et al, 2017). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Prior research on pedestrians' interaction with humandriven vehicles (HDVs) has highlighted the importance of nonverbal communication to ensure safety (Sucha et al, 2017;. Human drivers engage in nonverbal communication via eye contact, facial expressions, and hand gestures (Guéguen et al, 2015;.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Human drivers engage in nonverbal communication via eye contact, facial expressions, and hand gestures (Guéguen et al, 2015;. This is often done to communicate the drivers' intent when negotiating the right-of-way with pedestrians (Sucha et al, 2017). In the absence of a human driver, it is not surprising that pedestrians have expressed concerns over not knowing or understanding the AV's intention (Merat et al, 2018;Reig et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%