2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.11.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Post-capture processes contribute to statistical learning of distractor locations in visual search

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
54
1

Year Published

2021
2021
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
7
54
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A mixed-design ANOVA with the factors target condition and group revealed RTs to be significantly slower for targets appearing in the frequent versus the rare distractor region [1055 vs. 1035 ms; F (1, 42) = 4.67, p = 0.036, η p 2 = 0.10, BF incl = 1.67]—replicating previous studies using this paradigm (Wang and Theeuwes 2018a , b ; Zhang et al 2019 ). However, this target-position effect did not differ significantly between the ASD and TD groups [25 vs. 15 ms; F (1, 42) = 0.28, p = 0.60, BF incl = 0.34]—indicating that both groups acquired the same strategy of generally suppressing any singleton targets and distractors in the frequent distractor region, in line with suppression operating at the level of the search-guiding priority map (see Liesefeld and Müller 2019 , 2020 ; Sauter et al 2018 , 2019 ; 2020 ).
Fig.
…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…A mixed-design ANOVA with the factors target condition and group revealed RTs to be significantly slower for targets appearing in the frequent versus the rare distractor region [1055 vs. 1035 ms; F (1, 42) = 4.67, p = 0.036, η p 2 = 0.10, BF incl = 1.67]—replicating previous studies using this paradigm (Wang and Theeuwes 2018a , b ; Zhang et al 2019 ). However, this target-position effect did not differ significantly between the ASD and TD groups [25 vs. 15 ms; F (1, 42) = 0.28, p = 0.60, BF incl = 0.34]—indicating that both groups acquired the same strategy of generally suppressing any singleton targets and distractors in the frequent distractor region, in line with suppression operating at the level of the search-guiding priority map (see Liesefeld and Müller 2019 , 2020 ; Sauter et al 2018 , 2019 ; 2020 ).
Fig.
…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The behavioural signature of statistical distractor-location learning has been well documented recently: RT interference is reduced for distractors occurring at frequent versus rare locations, and this is associated with reduced capture of the first saccade by distractors at frequent locations (Di Caro et al 2019;Wang et al 2019a;Sauter et al 2020). Together with an ERP component interpreted in terms of distractor suppression (Wang et al 2019b), this has been taken as evidence that observers learn to down-modulate the attentional priority signals (Itti and Koch 2001;Fecteau and Munoz 2006;Wolfe and Gray 2007;Kok et al 2012;Aitken et al 2020) generated by distractors at frequent locations, thus reducing their potential to capture attention and cause interference.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Further of note, the distractor type was manipulated as a within-subject factor in the present study: our participants had to learn the spatial distribution of one type of distractor first and then, after an unlearning phase, the opposite distribution with the other type of distractor. In previous studies (Sauter et al 2018(Sauter et al , 2019(Sauter et al , 2020, we had used a between-subject design to avoid carry-over of acquired suppression strategies from one distractor type to the other. Based on finding dissociative target-location effects between same-and different-dimension distractors, we had proposed that statistical learning of distractor locations typically involves different levels of priority computation:…”
Section: Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Interestingly, while the effects of suppression history were visible for all kinds of distractors while distractor frequencies were manipulated, they were only found in the long term if this higher degree of perceptual/attentional processing had to be involved during the learning phase. The traces left in place during distractor filtering under these conditions are much more complex than those relative to their inhibition, and comprise a wealth of other types of processing, including a preliminary attentional selection based on their perceptual features 31 . Interestingly, recent studies have shown that in similar visual search tasks, if the perceptual features defining distractors are consistently used across trials (i.e., distractors appear more frequently in the same color or at the same location), the facilitation associated with their filtering is crucially linked with adaptations in the cortical activity of brain areas involved in visual processing as early as V1 59 , 60 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%