1998
DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60385-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prejudice and Stereotyping in Everyday Communication

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
51
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
1
51
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This would suggest that inconsistent information is communicated more than consistent information for its greater informativeness, although it has also been argued that stereotype-consistent information may be more informative than inconsistent information. Ruscher (1998) argued that although a group stereotype may be shared, any specific instance of how an individual fits a stereotype is new information, and therefore the new stereotype-consistent information may be seen to be informative in this sense.…”
Section: Communication Context: the Role Of Shared Knowledgementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This would suggest that inconsistent information is communicated more than consistent information for its greater informativeness, although it has also been argued that stereotype-consistent information may be more informative than inconsistent information. Ruscher (1998) argued that although a group stereotype may be shared, any specific instance of how an individual fits a stereotype is new information, and therefore the new stereotype-consistent information may be seen to be informative in this sense.…”
Section: Communication Context: the Role Of Shared Knowledgementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In dyadic conversation, communicators spend more time discussing stereotypeconsistent information, expressing agreement with stereotypeconsistent statements, and focusing questions and discussion on stereotype-consistent information (see Ruscher, 1998, for a review). When stereotype-relevant stories are passed along a chain of communicators, a stereotype consistency bias emerges in the communicated message as stereotype-consistent information is retained and inconsistent information left out (see Kashima, et al, 2007, for a review).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Over time, this process of mutual tuning and sayingis-believing may well produce group polarization regarding the stereotype (cf. Brauer et al, 2001;Ruscher, 1998). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They are then asked to discuss their impressions of the target. The results of content analyses of these dyadic conversations typically demonstrated more SC than SI utterances, both in terms of the number of comments and their duration (see Ruscher, 1998Ruscher, , 2001.…”
Section: Stereotypes and Communicationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They are then asked to discuss their impressions of the target. The results of content analyses of these dyadic conversations typically demonstrated more SC than SI utterances, both in terms of the number of comments and their duration (see Ruscher, 1998Ruscher, , 2001.The fi ndings by Ruscher and her colleagues are important because they have demonstrated that dyadic conversations are likely to concentrate on SC parts of stimulus information of an individual group member. However, a number of qualifi cations should be noted.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%