2017
DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01045
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reduced Left Lateralization of Language in Congenitally Blind Individuals

Abstract: Abstract■ Language processing depends on a left-lateralized network of frontotemporal cortical regions. This network is remarkably consistent across individuals and cultures. However, there is also evidence that developmental factors, such as delayed exposure to language, can modify this network. Recently, it has been found that, in congenitally blind individuals, the typical frontotemporal language network expands to include parts of "visual" cortices. Here, we report that blindness is also associated with re… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
50
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
(82 reference statements)
6
50
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Stevens and Weaver (2009) showed that early blind individuals exhibited reduced bold responses in their auditory cortex in response to tones, whereas, in contrast, Elbert et al (2002) showed that early blind individuals benefit from an expansion of the core tonotopic area of their auditory cortex. A pair of studies identified more subtle group laterality differences: Lane et al (2017) found stronger bilaterality for language processing in the congenitally blind, documenting a significantly reduced left-lateralized response for language stimuli, whereas Atilgan, Collignon, and Hasson (2017) found that cortical thickness measures in both auditory cortices were more strongly correlated with each other in the blind than in sighted individuals. The situation is quite similar in the somatosensory system, where only a handful of studies have documented changes within the primary sensory cortex of blind individuals (Pascual-Leone & Torres, 1993;Sterr et al, 1998aSterr et al, , 1998b.…”
Section: How Does Sensory Deprivation Affect the Intact Sensory Cormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Stevens and Weaver (2009) showed that early blind individuals exhibited reduced bold responses in their auditory cortex in response to tones, whereas, in contrast, Elbert et al (2002) showed that early blind individuals benefit from an expansion of the core tonotopic area of their auditory cortex. A pair of studies identified more subtle group laterality differences: Lane et al (2017) found stronger bilaterality for language processing in the congenitally blind, documenting a significantly reduced left-lateralized response for language stimuli, whereas Atilgan, Collignon, and Hasson (2017) found that cortical thickness measures in both auditory cortices were more strongly correlated with each other in the blind than in sighted individuals. The situation is quite similar in the somatosensory system, where only a handful of studies have documented changes within the primary sensory cortex of blind individuals (Pascual-Leone & Torres, 1993;Sterr et al, 1998aSterr et al, , 1998b.…”
Section: How Does Sensory Deprivation Affect the Intact Sensory Cormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, language-responsive "visual" areas are co-lateralized with the fronto-temporal language network, and show higher functional correlations with classical "language" regions even in the absence of a task (i.e. at rest) (Kanjlia, Lane, Feigenson, & Bedny, 2016;Bedny, et al, 2011;Lane et al, 2017;Liu et al, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consistent with this prior evidence for the malleability of the neural basis of language during development, evidence from studies of blindness suggests that the language network can be augmented with cortical real-estate that is typically occupied by visual perception. Individuals who are blind from birth recruit a network of "visual" areas during sentence processing, lexical retrieval, reading and word production tasks (Hamilton & Pascual-Leone, 1998;Kupers et al, 2007;Sadato et al, 1998;Bedny, Pascual-Leone, Dodell-Feder, Fedorenko, & Saxe, 2011;Lane, Kanjlia, Omaki, & Bedny, 2015;Lane et al, 2017;Röder, Stock, Bien, Neville, & Rösler, 2002;Watkins et al, 2012). This recruitment is part of a broader phenomenon, whereby in blindness, regions of the "visual" cortex are recruited for non-visual functions, including spatial localization and numerical cognition (for review, see Bedny, 2017;Collignon et al, 2011;Kanjlia, Lane, Feigenson, & Bedny, 2016;Kim, Kanjlia, Merabet, & Bedny, 2017;Röder, Rösler, & Neville, 2000).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, functional neural reorganization such as crossmodal plasticity has often been reported after sensory deprivation: for example, in the blind, ‘visual cortex’ can be activated by tactile stimulation (Büchel, 1998; Sadato et al, 1996), or auditory stimulation (Gougoux et al, 2005; Striem-Amit et al, 2012) or can even be recruited during symbolic math calculation (Kanjlia et al, 2016) or syntactic functions (Lane et al, 2017). In particular, a functional dissociation between a ventral “What” stream for the processing of object shape and a dorsal “Where” stream for the processing of space exists in blind subjects in both the auditory and tactile domain (Amedi et al, 2007; Collignon et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%