1988
DOI: 10.1016/0749-596x(88)90061-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sentence processing and lexical access: The influence of the focus-identifying task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
34
2

Year Published

1994
1994
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
3
34
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Admittedly, even when knowing the meaning of the prime and the visual target, less proficient learners might have had difficulties with the interpretation of the L2 sentences preventing the observation of reliable associative priming effects. In fact, associative priming effects depend on the comprehension and interpretation of the experimental sentence (Blutner & Sommer, 1988;Braun & Tagliapietra, 2009;Norris, Cutler, McQueen, & Butterfield, 2006;Tabossi, 1988;Williams, 1988). Assessing proficiency on the basis of the recognition of the filler trials tests the recognition of a large number of words and nonwords and provides an independent estimate of the non-native lexicon and L2 understanding.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Admittedly, even when knowing the meaning of the prime and the visual target, less proficient learners might have had difficulties with the interpretation of the L2 sentences preventing the observation of reliable associative priming effects. In fact, associative priming effects depend on the comprehension and interpretation of the experimental sentence (Blutner & Sommer, 1988;Braun & Tagliapietra, 2009;Norris, Cutler, McQueen, & Butterfield, 2006;Tabossi, 1988;Williams, 1988). Assessing proficiency on the basis of the recognition of the filler trials tests the recognition of a large number of words and nonwords and provides an independent estimate of the non-native lexicon and L2 understanding.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One way to encourage greater depth of processing on particular words in sentence contexts is to make them the semantic focus of their carrier sentences (Blutner & Sommer, 1988;Cutler & Fodor, 1979). Blutner and Sommer, for example, used cross-modal priming to study lexical ambiguity in the context of manipulations of sentence focus.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When attention is directed to a prime in one of these ways, priming can arise even for contextually inappropriate meanings. In Blutner and Sommer (1988), for example, no priming was observed at 0 ms ISI for either meaning of ambiguous primes when a previously presented question focussed listeners' attention away from the primes, but there was priming of responses to associates of both the appropriate and the inappropriate meanings of the primes when the primes were in focus. When a given word is brought more fully to the listener's attention as being relevant for ongoing utterance interpretation, the lexical semantics of that word can be activated more strongly, and associative priming may thus be found even for a meaning of that word that ultimately does not Wt into that interpretation.…”
Section: Conceptual Activationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The focus of the sentence may be indicated in a number of different ways. For example, focus has been implemented through the use of wh-questions (e.g., Birch & Rayner, 1997;Blutner & Sommer, 1988;Cutler & Fodor, 1979), there-insertion sentences (e.g., Birch & Garnsey, 1995), and it-cleft sentence constructions (e.g., Birch & Garnsey, 1995;Birch & Rayner, 1997). Focusing is thought to enhance the relative availability of concepts in memory in spoken language comprehension (Gernsbacher & Jescheniak, 1995;Gernsbacher & Shroyer, 1989) and in reading (Birch & Garnsey, 1995;Birch & Rayner, 1997;Carpenter & Just, 1977;Singer, 1976).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%