2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.07.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Statistical learning in the absence of explicit top-down attention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

10
68
3

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(81 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
10
68
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly consistent with our findings, some previous results indeed found that regularities that were implicitly learned remained in place even when the task changed or when the regularities were no longer present (e.g., Duncan & Theeuwes, 2020;Sauter et al, 2019). For example, showed that regularities regarding the location of the target learned during a training session generalized to a similar search tasks with different stimuli (i.e., from the T-among-Ls task to the 2-among-5s task) and with different difficulty (i.e., different offsets between two segments of nontarget "L" in different phases).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly consistent with our findings, some previous results indeed found that regularities that were implicitly learned remained in place even when the task changed or when the regularities were no longer present (e.g., Duncan & Theeuwes, 2020;Sauter et al, 2019). For example, showed that regularities regarding the location of the target learned during a training session generalized to a similar search tasks with different stimuli (i.e., from the T-among-Ls task to the 2-among-5s task) and with different difficulty (i.e., different offsets between two segments of nontarget "L" in different phases).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…For example, Jiang et al (2015) showed that regularities regarding the location of the target learned during a training session generalized to a similar search tasks with different stimuli (i.e., from the T-among-Ls task to the 2-among-5s task) and with different difficulty (i.e., different offsets between two segments of nontarget “L” in different phases). Duncan and Theeuwes (2020) showed that when participants performed two different tasks involving the same display, regularities that were present during one task stayed in place while performing another task. The learned spatial biases could persist more than one day even if when the spatial distribution was even (Jiang et al, 2013; Sauter et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our results are also consistent with those reported in related visual learning paradigms. For instance, Duncan and Theeuwes (2020) explored the persistence of attentional biases in a task where participants had to avoid searching in certain locations of the display because distractors appeared more frequently in that location. They found that this attentional bias disappeared after approximately 120 trials in which the spatial distribution of distractors was unbiased.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Critically, contingence between performance and outcomes was very large even when assuming that participants were insensitive to contingency changes (see Supplemental Material). It seems, however, unlikely that participants were unable to align expectations with changing outcome contingencies at least to some degree: People can implicitly track undisclosed contingencies (Duncan & Theeuwes, 2020; Kóbor et al., 2019), including those that depend on own response speed (Hwang & Kim, 2016). In our task, differences between incentive conditions were pointed out during instructions and condition‐specific feedback was presented in unique font colors (see Figure 1).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%