2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.05.107
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Taste perception and implicit attitude toward sweet related to body mass index and soft drink supplementation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
103
3
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 126 publications
(116 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
9
103
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Unlike the behavior observed with sweet orangeade, children did not increase their preference for the sour taste, which could be related to the fact that during childhood preferences for a sour taste are more stable than preferences for a sweet taste, and this agrees with some studies showing changes in sour preferences aer a long term exposure (eight months). 67 However, in a study performed by Sartor et al, 70 adults who disliked sucrose, who had a low score in a sucrose preference test performed with different concentrations of this sugar, increased their preference for a sweet taste aer a month in which they submitted to sweetened so drink supplementation. The important implication of this taste change is that continuous sweetened beverage consumption could determine an explicit preference change for a sweet taste.…”
Section: Development Of Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Unlike the behavior observed with sweet orangeade, children did not increase their preference for the sour taste, which could be related to the fact that during childhood preferences for a sour taste are more stable than preferences for a sweet taste, and this agrees with some studies showing changes in sour preferences aer a long term exposure (eight months). 67 However, in a study performed by Sartor et al, 70 adults who disliked sucrose, who had a low score in a sucrose preference test performed with different concentrations of this sugar, increased their preference for a sweet taste aer a month in which they submitted to sweetened so drink supplementation. The important implication of this taste change is that continuous sweetened beverage consumption could determine an explicit preference change for a sweet taste.…”
Section: Development Of Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Changes in taste sensitivity have been referred to in studies of obesity, especially regarding sweetness and bitterness. Although there is some controversy as to whether these are perceived with less or equal intensity by individuals with high body mass index (BMI) [12], most studies report that obese individuals present higher liking ratings for sweetness [40] and more pronounced aversive reactions to bitterness [17] than normal-weight subjects.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Past studies failed to show any association between sweet thresholds and nutritional status [8][9][10] , while more recent studies described a difference between overweight and normal-weight subjects [11,12] . In particular, it has been shown that PROP phenotype is related to body mass index (BMI) in females and that sweet (sucrose) as well as salty (sodium chloride) taste sensitivity are lower in young overweight/obese individuals compared with normal weight controls [13] . This suggests that overweight and obese subjects may have a reduced or distorted sensory sensitivity that might increase the desire and ingestion of food, thus leading to excessive energy intake and weight gain [14] .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%