2019
DOI: 10.1007/s40616-019-00113-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Teaching Mands for Information Using “Why” to Children With Autism

Abstract: Mands for information (MFIs) play an important role in language development and are important for successfully acquiring new information from one's environment. Yet many individuals with autism do not acquire mands for information without direct teaching. Research has demonstrated effective procedures for teaching all "wh" forms, except for "why." This study investigated procedures to teach the MFI "why" under control of the establishing operation and examined the extent to which teaching resulted in generaliz… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0
1

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
5
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The authors of 17 experiments reported assessments (e.g., Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Sparrow et al., 2005; VB‐MAPP, Sundberg, 2008; Preschool Language Scale, Zimmerman et al., 2002) to evaluate the participants' verbal behavior (Carnett et al., 2020; Carnett & Ingvarsson, 2016; Howlett et al., 2011; Kahlow et al., 2019; Landa et al., 2017, 2020; Lechago et al., 2010, 2013; Marion et al., 2011, Marion, Martin, Yu, Buhler, & Kerr, 2012; Pyles et al., 2021; Roy‐Wsiaki et al., 2010; Shillingsburg et al., 2018, 2019; Somers et al., 2014; Speckman et al., 2021; Valentino et al., 2019), and the authors of 12 experiments reported those assessment results (all but Howlett et al., 2011; Lechago et al., 2013; Shillingsburg et al., 2018; Somers et al., 2014). The authors of all 35 experiments reported anecdotal information for each participant's existing verbal repertoire (e.g., can tact more than 100 items; can mand for information using wh‐ questions, but does not mand for information using how?…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…The authors of 17 experiments reported assessments (e.g., Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Sparrow et al., 2005; VB‐MAPP, Sundberg, 2008; Preschool Language Scale, Zimmerman et al., 2002) to evaluate the participants' verbal behavior (Carnett et al., 2020; Carnett & Ingvarsson, 2016; Howlett et al., 2011; Kahlow et al., 2019; Landa et al., 2017, 2020; Lechago et al., 2010, 2013; Marion et al., 2011, Marion, Martin, Yu, Buhler, & Kerr, 2012; Pyles et al., 2021; Roy‐Wsiaki et al., 2010; Shillingsburg et al., 2018, 2019; Somers et al., 2014; Speckman et al., 2021; Valentino et al., 2019), and the authors of 12 experiments reported those assessment results (all but Howlett et al., 2011; Lechago et al., 2013; Shillingsburg et al., 2018; Somers et al., 2014). The authors of all 35 experiments reported anecdotal information for each participant's existing verbal repertoire (e.g., can tact more than 100 items; can mand for information using wh‐ questions, but does not mand for information using how?…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The authors of six experiments reported using indirect assessments (Marion, Martin, Yu, Buhler, & Kerr, 2012; Roy‐Wsiaki et al., 2010; Shillingsburg & Valentino, 2011; Patil et al., 2021; Williams et al., 2003), with one of those experiments (Williams et al., 2003) identifying objects that were aversive to the participants rather than reinforcing. Furthermore, in four experiments (Jessel & Ingvarsson, 2021; Landa et al., 2020; Valentino et al., 2019; Williams et al., 2000) the authors did not utilize preference assessments to identify reinforcing tangibles, edibles, or activities, and in two experiments (Carnett & Ingvarsson, 2016; Shillingsburg et al., 2018) the authors only provided information for mands for information.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations