1989
DOI: 10.3758/bf03204987
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of exposure duration and surrounding frames on direct and indirect tilt aftereffects and illusions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
18
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
1
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While increased adaptation time strengthens aftereffects, the interaction between adapting and test stimuli may not be fundamentally grounded in dynamic, adaptive processes. For example, repulsion in perceived orientation associated with the simultaneous tilt illusion (also reported by Gibson [12] closely resembles the tilt aftereffect in many respects, but does not entail adaptation [8], [32]. Furthermore, very brief presentations of an adaptor stimulus can in some cases lead to subsequent misperceptions in shape [33], [34] or orientation [35].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…While increased adaptation time strengthens aftereffects, the interaction between adapting and test stimuli may not be fundamentally grounded in dynamic, adaptive processes. For example, repulsion in perceived orientation associated with the simultaneous tilt illusion (also reported by Gibson [12] closely resembles the tilt aftereffect in many respects, but does not entail adaptation [8], [32]. Furthermore, very brief presentations of an adaptor stimulus can in some cases lead to subsequent misperceptions in shape [33], [34] or orientation [35].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…adapting) and probe stimuli (e.g. [8], for a review see [9]). These and other findings raise the question, to what extent are aftereffects determined by adaptation to the inspection stimulus, and to what extent are they a function of the relationship between the adapting and probe stimulus?…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Among them, the tilt illusion exhibits a similar temporal profile to our findings. Wenderoth and Johnstone (1988), using grating stimuli, showed that the tilt illusion decreased as test duration increased up to approximately 100 ms and then leveled off (see also Wenderoth & van der Zwan, 1989). Westheimer (1990) also showed a similar temporal profile of the tilt illusion using line stimuli.…”
Section: Other Illusionsmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…The brightness change in gray central bars by brighter/darker surrounding bars was seen at surround durations as short as 58 ms and was strongest at this duration. Other illusions such as the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet (COC) illusion (Wachtler & Wehrhahn, 1997), tilt contrast, and tilt aftereffect with flashed tests (Wenderoth & Johnstone, 1988;Wenderoth & van der Zwan, 1989;Westheimer, 1990;Wolfe, 1984) have been found to show similar duration-dependent properties. These fast-occurring illusions have one thing in common: They are all driven by a difference between adjacent areas on a local scale.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, when the orientation difference between the target and surround is large (60°-80°), the perceived target orientation is attracted toward that of the surround, a phenomenon known as the indirect TI (8,9). The indirect TI is smaller in magnitude and has been reported to differ in several fundamental ways from the TI (e.g., it is reduced by having a frame around the stimulus, it is immune to the addition of a gap between target and surround, and it is not dependent on spatial frequency) leading to the suggestion that it results from higherlevel feedback that reflects the operation of global processing and involves a separate mechanism to the one responsible for the TI (8)(9)(10).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%