2019
DOI: 10.1017/langcog.2019.14
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The iconicity toolbox: empirical approaches to measuring iconicity

Abstract: Growing evidence from across the cognitive sciences indicates that iconicity plays an important role in a number of fundamental language processes, spanning learning, comprehension, and online use. One benefit of this recent upsurge in empirical work is the diversification of methods available for measuring iconicity. In this paper, we provide an overview of methods in the form of a ‘toolbox’. We lay out empirical methods for measuring iconicity at a behavioural level, in the perception, production, and compre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
43
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
0
43
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For the iconicity ratings, people were asked to rate words on a scale that runs from -5 (anti-iconic or "words that sound like the opposite of what they mean") via 0 (arbitrary or "words that do not sound like what they mean or the opposite") to 5 (iconic or "words that sound like what they mean"). As Figure 1, panel B shows, the negative end of the scale was underused; subsequent analysis suggests that it was also used less consistently (Motamedi et al, 2019). The positive end of the scale successfully picked out words that show iconicity, defined (for spoken languages) as perceptual resemblances between aspects of word sound and meaning (Svantesson, 2017).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For the iconicity ratings, people were asked to rate words on a scale that runs from -5 (anti-iconic or "words that sound like the opposite of what they mean") via 0 (arbitrary or "words that do not sound like what they mean or the opposite") to 5 (iconic or "words that sound like what they mean"). As Figure 1, panel B shows, the negative end of the scale was underused; subsequent analysis suggests that it was also used less consistently (Motamedi et al, 2019). The positive end of the scale successfully picked out words that show iconicity, defined (for spoken languages) as perceptual resemblances between aspects of word sound and meaning (Svantesson, 2017).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With many sets of lexical ratings within easy reach, it is important to understand their affordances and limitations (Motamedi, Little, Nielsen, & Sulik, 2019). In sufficiently large datasets, almost any combination of lexical ratings will show some correlation.…”
Section: Dingemanse and Thompsonmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, the amount of sign iconicity may vary depending on how iconicity is operationalized, and there is some debate about how iconicity can best be objectively measured (see Motamedi, Little, Nielsen, & Sulik, 2019, for discussion). The ability to identify an iconic relationship between form and meaning depends on one’s linguistic and cultural experience (e.g., Occhino, Anible, Wilkinson, & Morford, 2017), and the perception of iconicity might be mediated by experience with linguistic patterns within the lexicon itself (Occhino, Anible, & Morford, 2020).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The difficulty in studying iconicity within an experimental context may be due in part to the multi-faceted nature of iconicity, making it a difficult parameter to operationalize (Motamedi, Little, Nielsen, & Sulik, 2019). While many theoretical papers in the European semiological tradition have examined, in depth, the nature of iconicity (Cuxac, 1996; Pietrandrea, 2002; Pizzuto & Volterra, 2000; Russo, 2004), within experimental paradigms investigating signed languages, iconicity is most frequently defined as a resemblance relationship between the form and the meaning of a linguistic unit (e.g., Bosworth & Emmorey, 2010; Ormel, Knoors, Hermans, & Verhoeven, 2009; Thompson, Vinson, & Vigliocco, 2009, 2010; Vinson, Cormier, Denmark, Schembri, & Vigliocco, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%