2016
DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1385-16.2016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Neural Dynamics of Attentional Selection in Natural Scenes

Abstract: The human visual system can only represent a small subset of the many objects present in cluttered scenes at any given time, such that objects compete for representation. Despite these processing limitations, the detection of object categories in cluttered natural scenes is remarkably rapid. How does the brain efficiently select goal-relevant objects from cluttered scenes? In the present study, we used multivariate decoding of magneto-encephalography (MEG) data to track the neural representation of within-scen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

22
109
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(132 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
22
109
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Distinction between an early, parallel processing stage and a later capacity-limited stage is central to most models of attention (Duncan, 1980;Treisman & Gelade, 1980). Target decoding in 3-item displays peaked at 252 ms with first significance at 196 ms, similar to attentional modulation of stimulus category processing in cluttered scenes observed from 180 ms (Kaiser et al, 2016), and to demonstration of feature-binding during integrated competition (Schoenfeld et al 2003). The later stage indexed by single-item decoding may correspond to capacity-limited individuation of the integrated target object, allowing its bound properties to become accessible for further processing and goal-directed action (Duncan, 1980;Bichot et al, 2005;Mitchell and Cusack 2008;Christie et al, 2015), in this case likely including the brightness judgement.…”
mentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Distinction between an early, parallel processing stage and a later capacity-limited stage is central to most models of attention (Duncan, 1980;Treisman & Gelade, 1980). Target decoding in 3-item displays peaked at 252 ms with first significance at 196 ms, similar to attentional modulation of stimulus category processing in cluttered scenes observed from 180 ms (Kaiser et al, 2016), and to demonstration of feature-binding during integrated competition (Schoenfeld et al 2003). The later stage indexed by single-item decoding may correspond to capacity-limited individuation of the integrated target object, allowing its bound properties to become accessible for further processing and goal-directed action (Duncan, 1980;Bichot et al, 2005;Mitchell and Cusack 2008;Christie et al, 2015), in this case likely including the brightness judgement.…”
mentioning
confidence: 81%
“…1,12 In this way, preparatory attention would bias visual processing in favor of objects sharing this attribute once the scene appears. 13 The existence of preparatory attention-related activity in the visual cortex has been relatively well established and accepted in doi: 10.1111/nyas.13320 the domain of spatial attention: focusing attention on a location in space is accompanied by increased baseline activity in parts of the visual cortex that are visually responsive to input at the attended location. 14,15 In daily life, however, our goals are typically related to nonspatial attributes, such as when looking out for cars or when searching for our keys.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Replicating previous reports, MEG sensor patterns could be used to accurately decode individual objects (Carlson et al, 2011;Carlson et al, 2013;van de Nieuwenhuijzen et al, 2013;Cichy et al, 2014;Isik et al, 2014;Clarke et al, 2015;Ritchie et al, 2015;Coggan et al, 2016;Kaiser et al, 2016a). Using representational similarity analysis, we then related MEG neural similarity to the objects' perceptual and categorical similarity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 80%