2017
DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000167
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The P3 Parietal-To-Frontal Shift Relates to Age-Related Slowing in a Selective Attention Task

Abstract: Abstract. Older adults recruit relatively more frontal as compared to parietal resources in a variety of cognitive and perceptual tasks. It is not yet clear whether this parietal-to-frontal shift is a compensatory mechanism, or simply reflects a reduction in processing efficiency. In this study we aimed to investigate how the parietal-to-frontal shift with aging relates to selective attention. Fourteen young and 26 older healthy adults performed a color Flanker task under three conditions (incongruent, congrue… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
22
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
3
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather than calculating peak amplitude and peak latency for the N450 and P3 components, the current study calculated mean amplitude given that: (a) older adults had no evident peaks in either component, as depicted by the grand-averaged waveforms ( Figure 1 ); (b) it could be problematic to use peak amplitudes when comparing young and older adults given that aging affects the onset and time course of N450 [ 23 ] and P3 [ 46 ]; and (c) calculating peak amplitude would be problematic considering the unequal number of congruent and incongruent trials in which the latter would have a lower signal-to-noise ratio [ 19 ], particularly for the N450 component. Clayson and colleagues [ 47 ] have validated the use of mean amplitude over peak amplitude or other adjusted methods, showing that the former provides robust ERP data regardless of changes in background artificial noise.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Rather than calculating peak amplitude and peak latency for the N450 and P3 components, the current study calculated mean amplitude given that: (a) older adults had no evident peaks in either component, as depicted by the grand-averaged waveforms ( Figure 1 ); (b) it could be problematic to use peak amplitudes when comparing young and older adults given that aging affects the onset and time course of N450 [ 23 ] and P3 [ 46 ]; and (c) calculating peak amplitude would be problematic considering the unequal number of congruent and incongruent trials in which the latter would have a lower signal-to-noise ratio [ 19 ], particularly for the N450 component. Clayson and colleagues [ 47 ] have validated the use of mean amplitude over peak amplitude or other adjusted methods, showing that the former provides robust ERP data regardless of changes in background artificial noise.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Likewise, the P3 mean amplitude was obtained by taking mean values within the 300- to 500-ms and 350- to 650-ms time windows after stimulus onset for young and older adults, respectively. These time windows were selected based on previous studies [ 19 , 46 , 48 , 49 ] and visual inspection of the grand-averaged waveforms ( Figure 1 ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We examined children’s cognitive performance (response accuracy and reaction time) during a modified flanker task as well as the peak amplitudes and latencies of the P3 elicited during the task. Abundant prior literature has used the flanker task to elicit P3 in many research contexts including investigations into conflict monitoring and inhibition (Clayson & Larson 2011a; Clayson & Larson 2011b; Purmann et al 2011; Groom & Cragg 2015), health-related behaviors (Hillman et al, 2014; Charles H. Hillman, Snook, & Jerome, 2003; Charles H Hillman, Buck, Themanson, Pontifex, & Castelli, 2009; Kamijo, Nishihara, Higashiura, & Kuroiwa, 2007; Pontifex & Hillman, 2007), child development (Brydges et al 2014; Rueda et al 2005; Ridderinkhof & van der Molen 1995; Ruberry et al 2016), cognitive aging (Hillman, Belopolsky, Snook, Kramer, & McAuley, 2004; Hsieh, Liang, & Tsai, 2012; Korsch, Frühholz, & Herrmann, 2016; Reuter, Voelcker-Rehage, Vieluf, Parianen Lesemann, & Godde, 2016), and clinical diagnoses (Johnstone & Galletta 2013; Johnstone et al 2009; Jonkman et al 1999; Liu et al 2011; Bartholow et al 2003; Rusnakova et al 2011; Racer et al 2011). We assessed lutein levels via a measure of Macular Pigment Optical Density (MPOD) using customized hetero-flicker photometry (cHFP).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With respect to aging, cue-P3 is delayed in older adults and exhibits less differentiation on the basis of cue significance (Hämmerer, Li, Müller, & Lindenberger, 2010;Kray, Eppinger, & Mecklinger, 2005;Kropotov, Ponomarev, Tereshchenko, Müller, & Jäncke, 2016). Similarly, target-P3 in older relative to younger adults is delayed, more frontally distributed, and is attenuated by conflict (Reuter, Voelcker-Rehage, Vieluf, Lesemann, & Godde, 2016;Wild-Wall, Falkenstein, & Hohnsbein, 2008;Williams et al, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%