2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.05.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding communicative intentions and semiotic vehicles by children and chimpanzees

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…At the time of testing, the participants had experienced few previous experimental tests, and none requiring object exchange. One chimpanzee, Maria-Magdalena, and one orangutan, Naong, had previously taken part in a planning experiment involving selection of items from a tray (Osvath and Osvath, 2008), and the chimpanzee Manda had experience of choice procedures from participation in an object-choice task (Zlatev et al, 2013). In addition, Naong had extensive experience of various object-choice procedures, requiring the selection of items ( unpublished ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At the time of testing, the participants had experienced few previous experimental tests, and none requiring object exchange. One chimpanzee, Maria-Magdalena, and one orangutan, Naong, had previously taken part in a planning experiment involving selection of items from a tray (Osvath and Osvath, 2008), and the chimpanzee Manda had experience of choice procedures from participation in an object-choice task (Zlatev et al, 2013). In addition, Naong had extensive experience of various object-choice procedures, requiring the selection of items ( unpublished ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It seems unlikely, however, that children had difficulty understanding the communicative act of using the markers as such, in light of previous research and in light of the present findings. On the one hand, previous research has shown that from 30 months or earlier, children understand markers as communicative gestures in similar tasks (Tomasello, Call, & Gluckman, ; Zlatev et al, ). And in the present study, children's above‐chance successful searches in the cue‐familiarization game (when only one cue was provided per trial) indicate that children understood the iconic markers as communicative gestures.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It seems unlikely, however, that children had difficulty understanding the communicative act of using the markers as such, in light of previous research and in light of the present findings. On the one hand, previous research has shown that from 30 months or earlier, children understand markers as communicative gestures in similar tasks (Tomasello, Call, & Gluckman, 1997;Zlatev et al, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Starting out from semiotics, however, I have argued that signs showing a direction are only a subcategory of those relying on contiguity (Sonesson 1989(Sonesson : 47, 1998. In an experiment (Zlatev et al 2013), we tried to separate mere contiguity from vectoriality (directionality), in the form of a marker and pointing, respectively. Interestingly, René Thom (1973) conceives indexicality in terms of the forward thrust of the arrow-head as imagined in water or the sentiment of its slipping from our hands.…”
Section: Indexicality and Directionmentioning
confidence: 99%