2006
DOI: 10.1080/17470210500416342
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual saliency and semantic incongruency influence eye movements when inspecting pictures

Abstract: Models of low-level saliency predict that when we first look at a photograph our first few eye movements should be made towards visually conspicuous objects. Two experiments investigated this prediction by recording eye fixations while viewers inspected pictures of room interiors that contained objects with known saliency characteristics. Highly salient objects did attract fixations earlier than less conspicuous objects, but only in a task requiring general encoding of the whole picture. When they were require… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

9
187
6
3

Year Published

2007
2007
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 222 publications
(205 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
9
187
6
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This definition of perceptual guidance is compatible with that of visual saliency (Itti & Koch, 2000;Parkhursta, Lawb, & Niebur, 2002). Saliency, as a composite measure of low-level visual information, is positively correlated with the presence of objects (Elazary & Itti, 2008), and found to guide attention in tasks where targets are underspecified (e.g., memorization, Underwood & Foulsham, 2006), and also utilized during situated language production (Gleitman et al, 2007). In our view, the most appropriate way to define visual saliency is in terms of visual clutter, calculated by integrating low-level visual information, e.g., color, with edge information (Rosenholtz, Mansfield, & Jin, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…This definition of perceptual guidance is compatible with that of visual saliency (Itti & Koch, 2000;Parkhursta, Lawb, & Niebur, 2002). Saliency, as a composite measure of low-level visual information, is positively correlated with the presence of objects (Elazary & Itti, 2008), and found to guide attention in tasks where targets are underspecified (e.g., memorization, Underwood & Foulsham, 2006), and also utilized during situated language production (Gleitman et al, 2007). In our view, the most appropriate way to define visual saliency is in terms of visual clutter, calculated by integrating low-level visual information, e.g., color, with edge information (Rosenholtz, Mansfield, & Jin, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…Half the images were then manipulated to add an 'incongruent' object (a small face) to them. The term 'incongruent' refers to the fact that the item is inserted in the image in a manner in which it would not typically occur (see Underwood & Foulsham, 2005). This is made clearer in Figure 1a.…”
Section: Materials and Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, researchers interested in attentional selection record where individuals look in scenes (e.g., Itti & Koch, 2000;Underwood & Foulsham, 2006). In addition, to better understand social attention, researchers have focused on biases in looking at social stimuli (e.g., Birmingham et al, 2008) and the characteristics of individual's looking behavior while engaged in social tasks (e.g., conversation; Keysar, Barr, Balin, & Brauner, 2000).…”
Section: Looking Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%