2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.jeem.2009.03.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What are the consequences of consequentiality?

Abstract: We offer an empirical test of a theoretical result in the contingent valuation literature. Specifically, it has been argued from a theoretical point of view that survey participants who perceive a survey to be "consequential" will respond to questions truthfully regardless of the degree of perceived consequentiality. Using survey data from the Iowa Lakes Project, we test this supposition. Specifically, we employ a Bayesian treatment effect model in which the degree of perceived consequentiality, measured as an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

12
107
3
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 150 publications
(124 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
12
107
3
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This finding contrasts the sharper empirical results in the existing literature (e.g. Herriges et al, 2010), but nevertheless highlights the importance of 30 consequentiality and the potential value in including pertinent survey questions. The evidence lends support to the view that the notion of consequentiality is more important than the "real vs.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 91%
“…This finding contrasts the sharper empirical results in the existing literature (e.g. Herriges et al, 2010), but nevertheless highlights the importance of 30 consequentiality and the potential value in including pertinent survey questions. The evidence lends support to the view that the notion of consequentiality is more important than the "real vs.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 91%
“…These results jibe well with nonexperianswering the value elicitation question. These results jibe well with nonexperimental evidence suggesting that surveys including explicit discussions on how the mental evidence suggesting that surveys including explicit discussions on how the results might infl uence policy produce different estimates than those that do not results might infl uence policy produce different estimates than those that do not (as in Herriges, Kling, Liu, and Tobias 2010). (as in Herriges, Kling, Liu, and Tobias 2010).…”
Section: Criterion Validity: Do Stated Preferences Estimates Match Resupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Herriges, tion we can make about how the respondent will answer the survey question. Herriges, Kling, Liu, and Tobias (2010) show that estimates of economic value from people who Kling, Liu, and Tobias (2010) show that estimates of economic value from people who received a consequentiality reminder are systematically different from those who did received a consequentiality reminder are systematically different from those who did not. However, empirical work on the effect of consequentiality scripts in stated prefernot.…”
Section: Lessons From Theory: When Should Stated Preference Estimatesmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In order for respondents to provide meaningful data, they need to believe that the survey is consequential and that their responses matter for policy purposes. At least two recent studies are designed to address consequentiality (Herriges et al 2010;Petrolia et al, 2014). Both appear to have fat tails, suggesting that a lack of Again, we see fat tails as a manifestation of hypothetical bias (the tendency of people to report a value other than their true value due to the hypothetical nature of a survey) and not an isolated contingent valuation issue.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%