1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03201210
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Nonstrategic subjective threshold effects in phonemic masking

Abstract: Three backward-masking experiments demonstrated that the magnitude of the phonemic mask reduction effect (MRE) is a function of subjective threshold and that the magnitude is also independent of stimulus-based response strategies. In all three experiments, a target word (e.g, bake) was backward masked by a graphemically similar nonword (e.g, BAWK), a phonemically similar nonword (e.g., BAlK), or an unrelated control (e.g., CRUG), Experiments 1 and 2 had a low percentage (9O,{,) of trials withphonemic masks and… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
25
0
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
3
25
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Once the stored representation has been activated, additional information about the exact pronunciation and spelling becomes available. In such a view, pre-lexical phonological coding is mandatory but the use of lexically supported phonology may be under strategic control (e.g., Berent & Perfetti, 1995;Frost, 1998;Gibbs & Van Orden, 1998;Xu & Perfetti, 1999). Berent (1997) directly addressed the issue of mandatory pre-lexical phonological assembly versus strategic reliance on post-lexical phonology by running a lexical decision task in which the target words were preceded by masked primes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Once the stored representation has been activated, additional information about the exact pronunciation and spelling becomes available. In such a view, pre-lexical phonological coding is mandatory but the use of lexically supported phonology may be under strategic control (e.g., Berent & Perfetti, 1995;Frost, 1998;Gibbs & Van Orden, 1998;Xu & Perfetti, 1999). Berent (1997) directly addressed the issue of mandatory pre-lexical phonological assembly versus strategic reliance on post-lexical phonology by running a lexical decision task in which the target words were preceded by masked primes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Using a backward masking paradigm, these authors only got a significant phonological masking effect when the majority of trials contained pseudohomophonic masks. Brysbaert and Praet's findings have been criticized on various grounds (e.g., Xu & Perfetti, 1999), but only recently has it become clear that phonological effects in Dutch are difficult to obtain with the backward masking paradigm and can be more easily observed with the masked priming technique (Brysbaert, 2000). Whatever the exact reason of Brysbaert and Praet's (1992) findings, they warn us that nothing sensible can be said about the extension of the phonological priming effect to the lexical decision task, if the effect has not first been replicated with the original naming task.…”
Section: Sand)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The prototype of this type of task is the masked priming paradigm. Using that paradigm, researchers have reported a net phonological priming effect under a variety of circumstances that would reduce the reliance on phonological information if the system had any strategic control on the use of this information (Berent, 1997;Brysbaert, 2001;Xu & Perfetti, 1999). Brysbaert et al (1999) reasoned that a strong phonological model of visual word recognition also has implications for bilingual language processing.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, Berent and Perfetti argued that the participants may have partly ignored the masks in the condition with a lot of unrelated masks (a criticism repeated by Ferrand, 1995). Two more criticisms were added by Xu and Perfetti (1999). First, they pointed out that the percentage of target recognition in the unrelated control condition was rather high in Brysbaert and Praet's experiments (more than 50%).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because participants in Verstaen et al's (1995) experiments were aware of the fact that a lot of the target words were homophones, many authors have considered this strategic effect as one that is comparable to the strategic effects in naming and lexical decision, the more so because the dependent variable (percentage correct target identification) did not allow on-line measurement of word processing (see, e.g., Xu & Perfetti, 1999). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%