2006
DOI: 10.1080/13506280500264460
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The spatial correspondence hypothesis and orienting in response to central and peripheral spatial cues

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

4
33
3

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
4
33
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with this assumption, in patients with unilateral spatial neglect, it is plausible that involuntary components mostly contribute to the characteristic disengage deficit for contralesional targets (Bonato, Priftis, Marenzi & Zorzi, 2009;Olk, Hildebrandt, & Kingstone, 2010). However, as already outlined here above, past studies primarily questioned the use of asymmetric symbolic, directional cues as arrows to induce endogenous spatial orienting (Lambert, Roser, Wells, & Heffer, 2006) without studying the role of cue predictivity. Nevertheless, indirect evidence suggests that predictivity might interact with expectations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 67%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Consistent with this assumption, in patients with unilateral spatial neglect, it is plausible that involuntary components mostly contribute to the characteristic disengage deficit for contralesional targets (Bonato, Priftis, Marenzi & Zorzi, 2009;Olk, Hildebrandt, & Kingstone, 2010). However, as already outlined here above, past studies primarily questioned the use of asymmetric symbolic, directional cues as arrows to induce endogenous spatial orienting (Lambert, Roser, Wells, & Heffer, 2006) without studying the role of cue predictivity. Nevertheless, indirect evidence suggests that predictivity might interact with expectations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 67%
“…The strength of the cueing effect on RTs therefore appears to be strongly modulated by the learning of cue/colortarget/location contingencies. In this specific case contingencies were arbitrary and therefore not apriori biased as opposed, for instance, to the use of asymmetrical cues like arrow cues (Lambert et al, 2006). Moreover, awareness of these contingencies (as indicated by post-experiment ratings) did not modulate the strength of the spatial orienting effect.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Frischen et al (2007) suggest that, given the recent findings with gaze and arrows, it may no longer be useful to divide spatial cueing into reflexive and voluntary categories (see also Lambert, Roser, Wells, & Heffer, 2006); rather, one might think in terms of a continuum of automaticity in spatial cueing tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It may be that very early cueing effects (i.e., 50 and 100 ms of SOA) are evoked only when the cue is asymmetric allowing spatial correspondence between central cue and target location to be automatically paired. Recent studies showed that spatial correspondence may help to develop fast orienting effects (Lambert & Duddy, 2002;Lambert, Roser, Wells, & Heffer, 2006) but further investigations are necessary to evaluate the link between learning mechanisms and cues asymmetry and their role in the attentional effects induced by eyes and arrow cues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%