1982
DOI: 10.1016/s1474-6670(17)62905-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Model of the Human Observer and Decision Maker — Theory and Validation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1982
1982
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As we have already assumed that the display is the only source of information for the user, it has to be designed to allow the user perceive, comprehend and predict the desired information to attain correct SA. Hence, the display design procedure requires a careful selection and clear presentation of the information for the user to perform the desired tasks correctly.Some examples which are available in the literature about incorporating human-factors into traditional control theory are (i) modelling the human operator as a controller for systems without [7-10] or with uncertainties [11,12], (ii) modelling the user as an estimator and Kalman filter [13,14] and (iii) modelling the user as an observer-based fault detector with the focus on modelling the decision making process [15,16]. Rather than modelling the user as a full-order Kalman-filter/observer, in [17] we modelled the user as a customised functional observer which could be personalised based on the capabilities of the operator.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As we have already assumed that the display is the only source of information for the user, it has to be designed to allow the user perceive, comprehend and predict the desired information to attain correct SA. Hence, the display design procedure requires a careful selection and clear presentation of the information for the user to perform the desired tasks correctly.Some examples which are available in the literature about incorporating human-factors into traditional control theory are (i) modelling the human operator as a controller for systems without [7-10] or with uncertainties [11,12], (ii) modelling the user as an estimator and Kalman filter [13,14] and (iii) modelling the user as an observer-based fault detector with the focus on modelling the decision making process [15,16]. Rather than modelling the user as a full-order Kalman-filter/observer, in [17] we modelled the user as a customised functional observer which could be personalised based on the capabilities of the operator.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, one could build upon the model proposed by Gai and Curry (1976), in which a generalized likelihood ratio (GLR) test is used to process optimal estimator residuals to determine the occurrence of a process failure event. Or, one could follow up on the modified GLR approach by Wewerinke (1981), or the discriminant analysis approach to discrete event detection proposed by Greenstein (1979). It suffices to note here that a range of options exist for modeling event detection performance, and for generating the desired event occurrence probabilities.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our eyes fixate features in the visual field, and the distribution of these fixations is usually not uniform (Papin, Naureils, & Santucci, 1980;Stark & Ellis, 1981). Several investigators (e.g., Wewerinke, 1981) have shown that oculomotor informationseeking during instrument monitoring can be modeled as random, or stratified random, sampling. Using similar analyses, other studies (e.g., Kapoula, 1983;Rayner & Pollatsek, 1981) have found structure in the transition patterns of scanning data, which may mean that subsequent fixations are being directed by repetitive, ongoing, cognitive processing.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%